2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.09.016
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Unconscious context-specific proportion congruency effect in a stroop-like task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
18
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
(60 reference statements)
5
18
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Mostly related to this study, in interference tasks the amplitude of N2 usually increases in high conflict condition relative to low conflict condition (Danielmeier, Wessel, Steinhauser, & Ullsperger, 2009;Forster, Carter, Cohen, & Cho, 2011;van Veen & Carter, 2002a), reflecting the detection of conflict in cognitive processing (Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004;Yeung et al, 2004). In addition, the N2 amplitude may be modulated, showing a similar conflict adaptation or PC effect as in the behavioral results (Clayson & Larson, 2011;Jiang, van Gaal, Bailey, Chen, & Zhang, Jiang et al 2013;Panadero, Castellanos, & Tudela, 2015), suggesting that the variation in the N2 amplitude may reflect the dynamic of cognitive control. These ERP results mirror the findings of fMRI studies (Blais & Bunge, 2010;Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter, & Cohen, 1999;Carter et al, 2000).…”
Section: N2 and Theta Oscillationsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Mostly related to this study, in interference tasks the amplitude of N2 usually increases in high conflict condition relative to low conflict condition (Danielmeier, Wessel, Steinhauser, & Ullsperger, 2009;Forster, Carter, Cohen, & Cho, 2011;van Veen & Carter, 2002a), reflecting the detection of conflict in cognitive processing (Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004;Yeung et al, 2004). In addition, the N2 amplitude may be modulated, showing a similar conflict adaptation or PC effect as in the behavioral results (Clayson & Larson, 2011;Jiang, van Gaal, Bailey, Chen, & Zhang, Jiang et al 2013;Panadero, Castellanos, & Tudela, 2015), suggesting that the variation in the N2 amplitude may reflect the dynamic of cognitive control. These ERP results mirror the findings of fMRI studies (Blais & Bunge, 2010;Botvinick, Nystrom, Fissell, Carter, & Cohen, 1999;Carter et al, 2000).…”
Section: N2 and Theta Oscillationsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…We would also note that our results do not go directly against more recent work on contextspecific conflict adaptation (e.g. Panadero et al, 2015;Reuss et al, 2014;Schouppe et al, 2014). In these studies participants can benefit from a conscious or unconscious cue indicating the context (high or low conflict) in which they are working, but must infer which context is which from the themselves.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 87%
“…The processing of the response congruency relation between prime and target has been previously associated with the N200 ERP component, a negative deflection with a fronto-central distribution that peaks around 250-350 ms after stimulus presentation (e.g., Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004). The amplitude of the N200 component has been found to be more negative in incongruent (or incompatible) trials compared to congruent (or compatible) trials (e.g., Clayson & Larson, 2011;Wendt & Luna-Rodríguez, 2009; see also Panadero, Castellanos, & Tudela, 2015). Furthermore, unconscious response priming has been shown to modulate occipito-parietal ERPs in the N200 time range, consistent with the notion of a rapid activation of visuo-motor representation within the dorsal visual pathway (e.g., Jaśkowski, Skalska, & Verleger, 2003;Martens, Ansorge, & Kiefer, 2011;Zovko & Kiefer, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%