2013
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00317
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Two separate processes affect the development of the mental number line

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
12
2

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
4
12
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This process was manifested as a modulation of the P3 congruency effect, thus it may be related to higher-order stimuli categorization and evaluation processes thought to be reflected in the P3 (e.g., Kutas et al, 1977 ; McCarthy and Donchin, 1981 ; Kok, 2001 ; Polich, 2007 , 2012 ). Future work is needed to better understand how the end-anchor identification process develops in relation to the analog comparison process ( Goldman et al, 2013 ) as well as characterizing the processing of less salient or prototypical end values by examining upper end values. We conclude that two processes are involved in the processing of numbers: an analog comparison process and an end-anchor identification process.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This process was manifested as a modulation of the P3 congruency effect, thus it may be related to higher-order stimuli categorization and evaluation processes thought to be reflected in the P3 (e.g., Kutas et al, 1977 ; McCarthy and Donchin, 1981 ; Kok, 2001 ; Polich, 2007 , 2012 ). Future work is needed to better understand how the end-anchor identification process develops in relation to the analog comparison process ( Goldman et al, 2013 ) as well as characterizing the processing of less salient or prototypical end values by examining upper end values. We conclude that two processes are involved in the processing of numbers: an analog comparison process and an end-anchor identification process.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Beyond the relevant FTT findings (e.g., Reyna, 2012; Reyna & Brainerd, 1991; Reyna et al, 2014), experimental data have shown that subjects prefer to avoid winning nothing in a risky gamble, even if doing so lowers their overall expected value (e.g., the “P max ” strategy of avoiding winning nothing as in Venkatraman & Huettel, 2012; Venkatraman, Payne, Bettman, Luce, & Huettel, 2009). In addition, recent findings show that zero may be encoded into an “end stimulus” category that is separate from how other numbers are encoded (Goldman, Tzelgov, Ben-Shalom, & Berger, 2013; Pinhas & Tzelgov, 2012; Wellman & Miller, 1986), leading Pinhas and Tzelgov (2012) to hypothesize that “0. .…”
Section: Section 3: the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Generally, when analyzing data, the last numerosity is excluded from the analysis due to its known end effect. However, the strong end effect found for numerosity 5 in Experiment 1 (and in Cohen et al.’s (2014) Experiment) might be due to the use of five neighboring fingers, which is the familiar pattern that results from real-world experience of counting with five fingers (see also Goldman, Tzelgov, Ben-Shalom, & Berger, 2013; Pinhas & Tzelgov, 2012). This suggestion can be tested by excluding a one-hand arrangement.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 79%