2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-011-0175-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Two (or three) is one too many: testing the flexibility of contextual cueing with multiple target locations

Abstract: Visual search for a target object is facilitated when it is repeatedly presented within an invariant context of surrounding items ('contextual cueing';Chun & Jiang, 1998). The current study investigated whether such invariant contexts can cue more than one target location. In a series of three experiments, we show that contextual cueing is significantly reduced when invariant contexts are paired with two rather than one possible target location, whereas no contextual cueing occurs with three distinct target lo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

11
42
4

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(57 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
11
42
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, contextual cueing was evident for landscape displays even after the insertion of two epochs of portrait displays. This may be taken to indicate that the cueing effect is relatively robust against interference within the same set of old configurations, consistent with previous studies (Chun & Jiang, 1998Conci & M¨uller, 2012;Jiang, Song, & Rigas, 2005;Zellin, Conci, von M¨uhlenen, & M¨uller, 2011). However, contextual cues acquired with landscape displays were transferred to portrait displays only under certain remapping conditions (those of Experiments 3 and 4), suggesting that contextual cueing is relatively inflexible and that transfer is confined to specific remapping situations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Interestingly, contextual cueing was evident for landscape displays even after the insertion of two epochs of portrait displays. This may be taken to indicate that the cueing effect is relatively robust against interference within the same set of old configurations, consistent with previous studies (Chun & Jiang, 1998Conci & M¨uller, 2012;Jiang, Song, & Rigas, 2005;Zellin, Conci, von M¨uhlenen, & M¨uller, 2011). However, contextual cues acquired with landscape displays were transferred to portrait displays only under certain remapping conditions (those of Experiments 3 and 4), suggesting that contextual cueing is relatively inflexible and that transfer is confined to specific remapping situations.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…In these cases, adaptation to relocated targets was not observed at all-not even when old contexts were rather permanently paired with relocated targets (Zellin et al, 2013), suggesting that only a single target location can be associated with an invariant context (Zellin et al, 2011). By contrast, here we observed contextual cueing for relocated targets when they were predictable from repeated encounters in the learning phase, specifically, when they were relocated from one old context to another old context (Experiment 1).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 46%
“…Reliable contextual cueing was not even observed when target relocations to previously empty positions were fairly permanent with at least twice as many presentations of the relocated targets relative to the initial target locations (Zellin, Conci, von Mühlenen, & Müller, 2013). This pattern of results suggests, on the one hand, that contextual cueing is essentially limited to single-target learning (Zellin, Conci, von Mühlenen, & Müller, 2011); that is, each old-context display can be associated with only one repeated target location (and its immediate surround; see also Makovski & Jiang, 2010), meaning that visual search for other (new) repeated target locations will not be guided by the same old context. On the other hand, the observed lack of contextual cueing for relocated targets appearing at previously empty positions might be owing to the fact that, after having learned a particular context, observers did not expect targets to appear at previously empty positions (Jiang et al, 2013;see Clark, 2013, for a theoretical approach to cognitive prediction models).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 45%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, incidental learning of statistical regularities is usually not adapted rapidly: an attentional bias toward a predicted target location (i.e., the most likely target location) is not rapidly readjusted to change and may persist for long periods of time (Jiang et al, 2012). Moreover, learning of context-target associations is typically limited to a single target location; no further target locations are associated with a given invariant context (Zellin et al, 2011). In addition, the adaptation of learned contextual associations after a change of the target location is rather inflexible, resulting in the persistence of a misleading cue (Manginelli and Pollmann, 2009).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%