2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Turn-taking in Human Communication – Origins and Implications for Language Processing

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

15
451
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 477 publications
(498 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
15
451
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants coordinate to put together their efforts in an optimal way, which involves correctly anticipating what one's partners will do and timing one's own actions, i.e., doing the right thing at the right time (Sebanz & Knoblich, 2009). Progress within the main body typically is accomplished via ad hoc turn-taking (Levinson, 2016). For example, two children engaging in pretend play might coordinate switching roles within the play session.…”
Section: Entering Into Maintaining and Dissolving Shared Intentionalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants coordinate to put together their efforts in an optimal way, which involves correctly anticipating what one's partners will do and timing one's own actions, i.e., doing the right thing at the right time (Sebanz & Knoblich, 2009). Progress within the main body typically is accomplished via ad hoc turn-taking (Levinson, 2016). For example, two children engaging in pretend play might coordinate switching roles within the play session.…”
Section: Entering Into Maintaining and Dissolving Shared Intentionalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, the smooth turn-taking occurring during conversation [52] strongly speaks for an autonomous and self-organizing interactive state, as discussed above [6].…”
Section: From One-person To Two-person Neurosciencementioning
confidence: 80%
“…These processes are so finely coordinated that interlocutors often minimize both overlap and gaps between turns. Indeed, Stivers et al (2009) found average inter-turn intervals between 0 and 200 ms in a comparison of 10 different languages, with overlap occurring only about 5% of the time (Levinson, 2016). Duncan (1972Duncan ( , 1974Duncan & Niederhe, 1974) proposed that interlocutors time their contributions during conversation by reacting to the presence of linguistic (e.g., drawl on the final syllable of the utterance) and nonlinguistic (e.g., termination of hand gestures) turn-yielding cues displayed at the end of the speaker's turn.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Current theories agree that interlocutors achieve such timings using prediction (De Ruiter, Mitterer, & Enfield, 2006;Garrod & Pickering, 2015;Levinson, 2016). Research has focused on how the listener can use such predictions to determine when the speaker will reach the end of their turn (e.g., De Ruiter et al, 2006;Magyari, Bastiaansen, De Ruiter, & Levinson, 2014) so they can articulate their response at the appropriate moment.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation