2013
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073590
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tuning of Human Modulation Filters Is Carrier-Frequency Dependent

Abstract: Recent studies employing speech stimuli to investigate ‘cocktail-party’ listening have focused on entrainment of cortical activity to modulations at syllabic (5 Hz) and phonemic (20 Hz) rates. The data suggest that cortical modulation filters (CMFs) are dependent on the sound-frequency channel in which modulations are conveyed, potentially underpinning a strategy for separating speech from background noise. Here, we characterize modulation filters in human listeners using a novel behavioral method. Within an ‘… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

5
14
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
(66 reference statements)
5
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Our data are consistent with experimental recordings from small mammals in which firing rates of auditory neurons adapt to the unfolding distributions of sound intensity (Ulanovsky et al, 2003(Ulanovsky et al, , 2004Nelken, 2004;Dean et al, 2005Dean et al, , 2008Peréz-González et al, 2005;Sadagopan and Wang, 2008;Watkins and Barbour, 2008;Malmierca et al, 2009;Wen et al, 2009;Barbour, 2011;Jaramillo and Zador, 2011;Rabinowitz et al, 2011;Walker and King, 2011;Yaron et al, 2012). This feature of neural coding, which emerges at the level of the primary auditory nerve, improves coding (discrimination) of the most-likely occurring intensities in a distribution of sounds (Dean et al, 2008).…”
Section: Neural Mechanismssupporting
confidence: 85%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Our data are consistent with experimental recordings from small mammals in which firing rates of auditory neurons adapt to the unfolding distributions of sound intensity (Ulanovsky et al, 2003(Ulanovsky et al, , 2004Nelken, 2004;Dean et al, 2005Dean et al, , 2008Peréz-González et al, 2005;Sadagopan and Wang, 2008;Watkins and Barbour, 2008;Malmierca et al, 2009;Wen et al, 2009;Barbour, 2011;Jaramillo and Zador, 2011;Rabinowitz et al, 2011;Walker and King, 2011;Yaron et al, 2012). This feature of neural coding, which emerges at the level of the primary auditory nerve, improves coding (discrimination) of the most-likely occurring intensities in a distribution of sounds (Dean et al, 2008).…”
Section: Neural Mechanismssupporting
confidence: 85%
“…These two demands appear in conflict in terms of their call on neural resources. Adaptation to "enhance" representation of both common (Dean et al, 2005(Dean et al, , 2008Sadagopan and Wang, 2008;Watkins and Barbour, 2008;Wen et al, 2009;Barbour, 2011;Jaramillo and Zador, 2011;Rabinowitz et al, 2011;Walker and King, 2011) and rare (Ulanovsky et al, 2003(Ulanovsky et al, , 2004Nelken, 2004;Peréz-González et al, 2005;Malmierca et al, 2009;Yaron et al, 2012) sounds has been reported in neurophysiological studies, seemingly in the same brain centers and using similar probabilistic stimulus paradigms. How then does sensitivity to the statistical distribution of sounds manifest in sensitivity to both high-probability and low-probability events?…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 80%
See 3 more Smart Citations