2016
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-016-1041-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Triadic awareness predicts partner choice in male–infant–male interactions in Barbary macaques

Abstract: Social knowledge beyond one's direct relationships is a key to successful maneuvering of the social world. Individuals gather information on the quality of social relationships between their group companions, which has been termed triadic awareness. Evidence of the use of triadic awareness in natural contexts is limited mainly to conflict management. Here we investigated triadic awareness in wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) in the context of bridging interactions defined as male-infant-male interactions… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
21
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 92 publications
2
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Under these circumstances, species should evolve mechanisms that allow individuals to adapt their behaviour to the audience (Bshary and Noë 2003). Whether individuals decide to interact at all to signal their ability to cooperate (cleaner fish: Bshary and Grutter 2006;Pinto et al 2011), change the form of interactions to increase the likelihood of being chosen as a partner (Betta splendens: Dzieweczynski et al 2005 or adapt their partner choices to optimize their own fitness benefits (longtailed macaques: Gumert 2007; white-faced capuchins: Kajokaite et al 2019; Barbary macaques: Kubenova et al 2016; sooty mangabeys and chimpanzees: Mielke et al 2018;Atlantic molly: Plath et al 2008, 2009, the presence and composition of audiences can influence animal behaviour in important ways. There are increasingly detailed accounts of animals altering their behaviour in response to specific aspects of the relationships between potential interaction partners and observing bystanders (vervet monkeys: Borgeaud et al 2017;baboons: Wittig et al 2007;chimpanzees: Wittig et al 2014; Kaburu and Newton-Fisher 2016;Mielke et al 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Under these circumstances, species should evolve mechanisms that allow individuals to adapt their behaviour to the audience (Bshary and Noë 2003). Whether individuals decide to interact at all to signal their ability to cooperate (cleaner fish: Bshary and Grutter 2006;Pinto et al 2011), change the form of interactions to increase the likelihood of being chosen as a partner (Betta splendens: Dzieweczynski et al 2005 or adapt their partner choices to optimize their own fitness benefits (longtailed macaques: Gumert 2007; white-faced capuchins: Kajokaite et al 2019; Barbary macaques: Kubenova et al 2016; sooty mangabeys and chimpanzees: Mielke et al 2018;Atlantic molly: Plath et al 2008, 2009, the presence and composition of audiences can influence animal behaviour in important ways. There are increasingly detailed accounts of animals altering their behaviour in response to specific aspects of the relationships between potential interaction partners and observing bystanders (vervet monkeys: Borgeaud et al 2017;baboons: Wittig et al 2007;chimpanzees: Wittig et al 2014; Kaburu and Newton-Fisher 2016;Mielke et al 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, in Barbary macaques this stability may be beneficial in relation to the high frequency of man–infant–man interactions, during which two males manipulate one infant together and which help males to manage relationships with other males (Kalbitz et al, ; Kuběnová et al, ; Paul et al, , Taub ). The stable pattern may reduce competition over infants used in these interactions (Kalbitz et al, ; Paul et al, ) and make those interactions more effective tools for male relationship management (Kubenova et al, ; Ogawa, ; Paul et al, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In accordance with the 'extended mating effort hypothesis', however, (Kalbitz et al, 2017;Kuběnová et al, 2019;Paul et al, 1996, Taub 1980b). The stable pattern may reduce competition over infants used in these interactions (Kalbitz et al, 2017;Paul et al, 1996) and make those interactions more effective tools for male relationship management (Kubenova et al, 2017;Ogawa, 1995;Paul et al, 1996).…”
Section: Model 4: Mating Effort Hypothesis Male Perspectivementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…triadic behaviors have been recorded in Barbary (M. sylvanus; Deag and Crook 1971;Taub 1984;Paul et al 1996;Kubenova et al 2017), stump-tailed (M. arctoides; Estrada and Sandoval 1977;Estrada and Estrada 1984), Assamese (M. assamensis; Kubenova et al 2017), and Tibetan macaques (M. thibetana; Ogawa 1995a, b, c;Zhao 1996;Bauer et al 2013). Adult males' bridging tends to occur in non-agonistic contexts and is often followed by other affiliative behaviors such as grooming (Ogawa 1995a, b;Deag and Crook 1971).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%