2013
DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00090
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transitions between discrete and rhythmic primitives in a unimanual task

Abstract: Given the vast complexity of human actions and interactions with objects, we proposed that control of sensorimotor behavior may utilize dynamic primitives. However, greater computational simplicity may come at the cost of reduced versatility. Evidence for primitives may be garnered by revealing such limitations. This study tested subjects performing a sequence of progressively faster discrete movements in order to “stress” the system. We hypothesized that the increasing pace would elicit a transition to rhythm… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
24
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
4
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, the decrement in movement amplitude and hastening that occurs near to and above 2 Hz in people with PD may reflect an inability to transition coordination patterns with an accompanying loss of movement stability. Two movements per second is also the rate at which movements typically change from discrete to continuous control (Huys et al, 2008; Sternad et al, 2013). Spencer and colleagues (2007) have hypothesized that discrete movement timing is predominantly controlled by the cerebellum whereas continuous movement timing may be dependent upon the basal-ganglia.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, the decrement in movement amplitude and hastening that occurs near to and above 2 Hz in people with PD may reflect an inability to transition coordination patterns with an accompanying loss of movement stability. Two movements per second is also the rate at which movements typically change from discrete to continuous control (Huys et al, 2008; Sternad et al, 2013). Spencer and colleagues (2007) have hypothesized that discrete movement timing is predominantly controlled by the cerebellum whereas continuous movement timing may be dependent upon the basal-ganglia.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Different authors have considered MPs at several levels. MPs have been defined as kinematic elements (i.e., patterns of motion without regard to force or mass, e.g., strokes [1-3] or cycles [4,5]), or as kinetic elements (force-based, e.g., viscoelastic force-field primitives [6-9] and their associated muscle synergies[9-11]), or as neural drive circuits [12]. Because the nervous system appears to organize both the executed motion as well as prepare the contingent acts and corrections required for any common or unexpected perturbations in any complex body motion, it follows that the most fundamental MPs need to be useful as compositional elements in each of these contexts, i.e., the fundamental MPs must be adequate building blocks to rapidly construct the kinematic, kinetic, impedance and contingent response aspects of a complex movement.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Transitions from fast discrete movements to rhythmic movement (Sternad et al, 2013 ) as well as transitions from slowly continuous movement to sub-movements (Teeken et al, 1996 ; van der Wel et al, 2009 ) reveal that the underlying controls for discrete and rhythmic movements are based on the same modules. Our results provide further evidence that muscular synergies underlying both types of movement are the same.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%