2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.04.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transfer of strategy use by semantic recoding in arithmetic problem solving

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
43
0
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
(29 reference statements)
1
43
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Our materials were inspired by Gamo et al (2010), who showed that problems with the same formal mathematical structure are nevertheless preferentially solved with one of two available solving strategies, depending on the semantic content of the problem. Consider the weight problem in Table 1: this problem can be solved through two strategies.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our materials were inspired by Gamo et al (2010), who showed that problems with the same formal mathematical structure are nevertheless preferentially solved with one of two available solving strategies, depending on the semantic content of the problem. Consider the weight problem in Table 1: this problem can be solved through two strategies.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The authors demonstrated that slight, mathematically-irrelevant changes in the semantic relations linking the objects mentioned in the cover stories (e.g., computers assigned to secretaries vs. secretaries assigned to computers) led to significant performance differences. Subsequent research has shown that non-mathematical semantic information related to the entities described in a problem influences lay solvers' performance (Bassok, Chase, & Martin, 1998;Gros, Sander, & Thibaut, 2016;Thevenot & Barrouillet, 2015;Verschaffel, De Corte, & Vierstraete, 1999;Vicente, Orrantia, & Verschaffel, 2007) as well as strategy choice (Gamo, Sander, & Richard, 2010;Gros, Thibaut, & Sander, 2017) and transfer (Gros, Thibaut, & Sander, 2015) on arithmetic word problems. Most of the available evidence regarding this issue has been collected with children and non-expert adults on problems that were not straightforward (e.g., complex permutation problems).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Especially demanding is the "knowledge compilation" mechanism involved in transfer, which "operates like a translation device, interpreting prior declarative knowledge into a set of procedures or cognitive rules to perform a specific task… [and]…can cause performance errors because of the working memory limitations that constrain how much declarative information can be retrieved" (Nokes-Malach & Mestre, 2013, p. 187). Researchers infer transfer from scores in the transfer task in measures such as recall (O'Sullivan & Pressley, 1984), number of analogies produced (Brown & Kane, 1988), or number of problems solved (Gamo, Sander, & Richard, 2010). However, a more precise assessment of transfer requires measuring not only these performance scores but also the degree of transfer of the strategy (or rule, etc.).…”
Section: Transfer Of Cognitive Strategies In Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Die Möglichkeit des Strategietransfers zwischen unterschiedlichen Aufgaben wurde in der Geometrie von Chinnappan und Lawson (1996) und für die Arithmetik von Gamo et al (2010) untersucht und bestätigt. In der genannten Studie zum Strategietransfer im Bereich Geometrie wurde anhand eines Kontrollgruppendesigns der Einfluss eines Strategietrainings, das aus den Elementen Wiederholendes Lesen, Planung und Kontrolle bestand, auf die Testleistungen von Schülern bei der Bearbeitung von mathematischen Aufgaben untersucht.…”
Section: Modellierungskompetenzunclassified