2013
DOI: 10.1002/dev.21109
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Transfer of spatial search between environments in human adults and young children (Homo sapiens): Implications for representation of local geometry by spatial systems

Abstract: Use policyThe full-text may be used and/or reproduced, and given to third parties in any format or medium, without prior permission or charge, for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-prot purposes provided that:• a full bibliographic reference is made to the original source • a link is made to the metadata record in DRO • the full-text is not changed in any way The full-text must not be sold in any format or medium without the formal permission of the copyright holders.Please consult the full D… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

3
30
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
3
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, participants displayed a significant preference for navigating to the correct corner first during a test trial. Together, these results are consistent with other experiments that have demonstrated similar navigational transfer effects across environments of different shapes ( Lew et al, 2014 ; McGregor et al, 2006 ; Pearce et al, 2004 ; Tommasi & Polli, 2004 ), and are consistent with the idea that, during training, participants used a local geometric-cue in order to find the hidden goal. For example, participants may have learned during training that approaching an egocentrically encoded scene, such as the conjunction of two walls of different lengths, was associated with the goal ( Cheung, Stürzl, Zeil, & Cheng, 2008 ; McGregor et al, 2006 ; Pearce et al, 2004 ; Stürzl, Cheung, Cheng, & Zeil, 2008 ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 91%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Moreover, participants displayed a significant preference for navigating to the correct corner first during a test trial. Together, these results are consistent with other experiments that have demonstrated similar navigational transfer effects across environments of different shapes ( Lew et al, 2014 ; McGregor et al, 2006 ; Pearce et al, 2004 ; Tommasi & Polli, 2004 ), and are consistent with the idea that, during training, participants used a local geometric-cue in order to find the hidden goal. For example, participants may have learned during training that approaching an egocentrically encoded scene, such as the conjunction of two walls of different lengths, was associated with the goal ( Cheung, Stürzl, Zeil, & Cheng, 2008 ; McGregor et al, 2006 ; Pearce et al, 2004 ; Stürzl, Cheung, Cheng, & Zeil, 2008 ).…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 91%
“…One test trial was conducted in an arena that had walls the same color as the arena in which participants were trained. If participants transfer the local shape-information from the training to the test arena then they should preferentially search in the corner(s) of the test arena that match the local geometric features of the training arena ( Lew et al, 2014 ; McGregor et al, 2006 ; Pearce et al, 2004 ). A second test trial was conducted in which the test walls were a different color to the training walls.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For TD children and adults, the process of combining features with geometry appears to operate seamlessly and in parallel at the behavioral level. When adults re-establish orientation, they likely establish their current heading direction by comparing it to a stored global representation of the spatial layout (Lew et al, 2014). Like adults, young children appear to combine geometry with features efficiently, presumably comparing their current heading to a stored geometric representation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pigeons searched there more than chicks, which led the authors to the conclusion that pigeons relied primarily on medial axes, whereas chicks primarily used local geometry followed by medial axes. There is also evidence that humans are able to use the principal axis (Ambosta, Reichert, & Kelly, 2013;Bodily, Eastman, & Sturz, 2011) and that they have both local and global representations available to them (Buckley, Smith, & Haselgrove, 2016;Lew et al, 2014). Of interest, Figure 4.…”
Section: P R E -P U B L I C a T I O N P R O O F Pre-publication Proofmentioning
confidence: 99%