2009
DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.2.588
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Trajectories emerging from discrete versus continuous processing models in phonological competitor tasks: A commentary on Spivey, Grosjean, and Knoblich (2005).

Abstract: M. J. Spivey, M. Grosjean, and G. Knoblich showed that in a phonological competitor task, participants' mouse cursor movements showed more curvature toward the competitor item when the competitor and target were phonologically similar than when the competitor and target were phonologically dissimilar. Spivey et al. interpreted this result as evidence for continuous cascading of information during the processing of spoken words. Here we show that the results of Spivey et al.need not be ascribed to continuous sp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
29
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
1
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The overall dynamics of the movement is largely due to the two fast movements (ξ = 2.94, 91 %) with only a motor pause for the competing category (ζ 1 = 0.28, 9 %; ζ 2 = 0, 0 %). This profile is an example of a movement path composed by two discrete sub-movements separated by a pause (e.g., see: Hwang et al, 2005;van der Wel, Eder, Mitchel, Walsh, & Rosenbaum, 2009), which is difficult to reconcile with the assumption of continuous movement trajectories assumed by some of the DGA interpretations (e.g., see: J. Freeman, Dale, & Farmer, 2011).…”
Section: Data Analysis and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The overall dynamics of the movement is largely due to the two fast movements (ξ = 2.94, 91 %) with only a motor pause for the competing category (ζ 1 = 0.28, 9 %; ζ 2 = 0, 0 %). This profile is an example of a movement path composed by two discrete sub-movements separated by a pause (e.g., see: Hwang et al, 2005;van der Wel, Eder, Mitchel, Walsh, & Rosenbaum, 2009), which is difficult to reconcile with the assumption of continuous movement trajectories assumed by some of the DGA interpretations (e.g., see: J. Freeman, Dale, & Farmer, 2011).…”
Section: Data Analysis and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hence, it might also be interesting for future research within the field of decision making to pay more attention to individual differences. Finally, we speculate that these individual differences are correlated with personality-related factors, such as, for example, sensation seeking (Zuckerman, 1979) and state/trait anxiety (Spielberger, 1985). Specifically, one might expect discrete guessing to be associated with high sensation seeking/low anxiety and holding back and, to a lower degree, one might expect equal parallel attraction behavior to be associated with low sensation seeking/high anxiety.…”
Section: Comparing Anticipation In the Face Of Semicertainty To Anticmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…To explain these findings, several researchers have developed dynamical models that incorporate competitive processes. For example, [40] implemented a dynamic competitive model of choice that successfully explains the results of a "phonological competitor" task [41] (but see [42] for an alternative explanation that consider perception as a serial rather than parallel process).…”
Section: F Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%