2015
DOI: 10.1515/revneuro-2014-0078
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tracking markers of response inhibition in electroencephalographic data: why should we and how can we go beyond the N2 component?

Abstract: Response inhibition is a pivotal component of executive control, which is especially difficult to assess. Indeed, it is a substantial challenge to gauge brain-behavior relationships because this function is precisely intended to suppress overt measurable behaviors. A further complication is that no single neuroimaging method has been found that can disentangle the accurate time-course of concurrent excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms. Here, we argue that this objective can be achieved with electroencephalogra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

2
29
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(31 citation statements)
references
References 118 publications
2
29
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This speculation is consistent with a growing literature that argues for blurred distinctions between automatic and controlled processing in elite athletic performance (see discussion in Bashore et al, 2018). However, on the basis of findings from a meta-analysis, Albares et al (2015) offered a set of penetrating methodological, technical, and analytical criticisms to argue that the N200 does not serve as either a reliable or valid index of response inhibition. With these criticisms in mind, our view is that the pattern of ERP activity observed by Nakamoto and Mori (2012) suggests that players recognize target movement deceleration more quickly (N200 latency) and re-calibrate the level of activation of the prepared movement to it more effectively (P300 amplitude) than do controls, while concurrently slowing translation of the stimulus input (i.e., change in target speed) into the designated response output (button press) until the time of target arrival with greater proficiency than do controls (P300 latency; see Verleger et al, 2005;Bashore et al, 2013, for discussions of the putative relationship between P300 latency and S-R translation).…”
Section: Enhanced Executive Control Systems In Elite Reactive/intercesupporting
confidence: 67%
“…This speculation is consistent with a growing literature that argues for blurred distinctions between automatic and controlled processing in elite athletic performance (see discussion in Bashore et al, 2018). However, on the basis of findings from a meta-analysis, Albares et al (2015) offered a set of penetrating methodological, technical, and analytical criticisms to argue that the N200 does not serve as either a reliable or valid index of response inhibition. With these criticisms in mind, our view is that the pattern of ERP activity observed by Nakamoto and Mori (2012) suggests that players recognize target movement deceleration more quickly (N200 latency) and re-calibrate the level of activation of the prepared movement to it more effectively (P300 amplitude) than do controls, while concurrently slowing translation of the stimulus input (i.e., change in target speed) into the designated response output (button press) until the time of target arrival with greater proficiency than do controls (P300 latency; see Verleger et al, 2005;Bashore et al, 2013, for discussions of the putative relationship between P300 latency and S-R translation).…”
Section: Enhanced Executive Control Systems In Elite Reactive/intercesupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Guidelines for assessing the cortical brain dynamics of response inhibition with EEG are summarized in Albares et al . (). In sum, we compared the mean power of the 500 ms pre‐stimulus period to the mean power of the 100 ms pre‐RT period on a single‐trial basis (aggregate data) by means of Kruskal–Wallis tests and post hoc testing ( p < 0.05 Tukey‐Cramer corrected).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…), (ii) apply group Blind Source Separation (gBSS), a method that directly identifies the components that are consistently expressed in the population (Lio and Boulinguez ; Albares et al . ; Huster et al . ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We quantified the P2 in the time window between 170 and 200 ms in GO and NOGO trials for the “stimulation group” and 210–240 ms in GO and NOGO trials for the “no‐stimulation group.” The mean amplitude in the N2 time window was quantified at electrode Cz in the time range from 350 to 380 ms in GO trials for both groups and time points. This time window might seem late for this component, yet the N2 component can occur between 200 and 400 ms after stimulus onset (Albares, Lio, & Boulinguez, ) and the chosen time window is still within this range. In NOGO trials, the time window from 260 to 290 ms was used for the “stimulation group” for both time points.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%