2018
DOI: 10.1177/1747021817739861
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tracing the path of forgetting in rule abstraction and exemplar retrieval

Abstract: One of the earliest discovered laws in psychology is the law of forgetting. The more time has passed between encoding an item and retrieving this item, that is, the longer the retention interval, the less likely people recall the item correctly (Ebbinghaus, 1885; Rubin & Wenzel, 1996). On a class reunion 1 year after high school, for instance, the names of former classmates may easily come to your mind. After 20 years, however, you may even encounter problems when naming your former best friends. The course of… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
(128 reference statements)
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, in the study by Hoffmann et al (2014), exemplars were presented 10 times before participants entered the judgment phase. The use of exemplar memory becomes more likely, the more accurately participants can recall the exemplars (Hoffmann et al, 2014; Hoffmann et al, 2018; Johansen & Palmeri, 2002). This enhancement may be explained through a more distinct memory representation (Rouder & Ratcliff, 2004) of the seen exemplars or by a larger number of stored memory traces for exemplars (Dougherty et al, 1999; Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997).…”
Section: Lan and Memory Retrievalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, in the study by Hoffmann et al (2014), exemplars were presented 10 times before participants entered the judgment phase. The use of exemplar memory becomes more likely, the more accurately participants can recall the exemplars (Hoffmann et al, 2014; Hoffmann et al, 2018; Johansen & Palmeri, 2002). This enhancement may be explained through a more distinct memory representation (Rouder & Ratcliff, 2004) of the seen exemplars or by a larger number of stored memory traces for exemplars (Dougherty et al, 1999; Nosofsky & Palmeri, 1997).…”
Section: Lan and Memory Retrievalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The exemplar model we use as an example in this paper is based on the context model of Medin and Schaffer ( 1978 ) extended to account for the continuous criterion in multiple-cue judgments (Juslin et al, 2003 ). This and similar exemplar models have been used in many studies of multiple-cue judgments, where it is assumed that judgments are based on the memory of previously encountered exemplars (e.g., Bröder & Gräf, 2018 ; Bröder, Gräf, & Kieslich, 2017 ; Juslin et al, 2003 ; Hoffmann et al, 2013 ; Hoffmann et al, 2014 ; Hoffmann, von Helversen, Weilbächer, & Rieskamp, 2018 ; Karlsson et al, 2008 ; Platzer & Bröder, 2013 ; von Helversen & Rieskamp, 2008 ; von Helversen, Mata, & Olsson, 2010 ; Wirebring et al, 2018 ). According to this model, when a judgment is made about a probe (i.e., a stimulus that has to be judged), the judge considers the similarity of the probe to all of the previously encountered exemplars.…”
Section: Exemplar Model Used In Multiple-cue Judgment Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, rule-based learning (combined cues with an abstract principle) has been reported during associative learning both in human and animal studies (Racht-Delatour and Massioui, 1999;Wills et al, 2011;Maes et al, 2017;Broschard et al, 2019). Compared with remembering only separate cues, rule-based learning is more stable with the passing of time and can adjust to various contexts (Hoffmann et al, 2018). That is, the strategies to solve problems obtained from our experience are more reliable in memory and can facilitate subsequent new learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%