2005
DOI: 10.1075/sl.29.2.04vri
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Towards a typology of tail–head linkage in Papuan languages

Abstract: In this article a typological overview of tail–head linkage (THL) in Papuan languages is presented. There are two types of THL, chained THL and thematized THL. The chained type is the default type and its morphosyntactic form follows from the basic clause linkage type in a given Papuan language, for example when switch reference constructions are the basic type of clause linkage, then the default type of THL takes the form of switch reference constructions. Chained THL carries referential coherence mechanisms … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
19
0
7

Year Published

2009
2009
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
19
0
7
Order By: Relevance
“…This is unexpected since the -t (with allomorph -r in Mandobo, see Drabbe 1959: 32) is a marker of Realis status in South and North Wambon. The explanation for t-forms in conditional contexts could be that what Drabbe calls a conditional is in fact a generic thematised clause construction of a type found in very many Papuan languages that can have many interpretations, including conditional readings (see De Vries 2005;2006). These generic clausal thematic NPs should be glossed as 'given that X, Y' and do not at all require special Conditional or Irrealis verb forms.…”
Section: Semi-finite Verbsmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This is unexpected since the -t (with allomorph -r in Mandobo, see Drabbe 1959: 32) is a marker of Realis status in South and North Wambon. The explanation for t-forms in conditional contexts could be that what Drabbe calls a conditional is in fact a generic thematised clause construction of a type found in very many Papuan languages that can have many interpretations, including conditional readings (see De Vries 2005;2006). These generic clausal thematic NPs should be glossed as 'given that X, Y' and do not at all require special Conditional or Irrealis verb forms.…”
Section: Semi-finite Verbsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…To distribute the perceived event to a separate conjoined or chained clause (he sees that a pig comes>he sees and a pig comes) is a normal perception construction in Papuan languages with distributive tendencies, see De Vries 2005;2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Typically, sentence beginnings in narratives are marked by one or more clauses which recapitulate the events denoted at the end of the previous sentence (Genetti 2007: 438-439). This discourse pattern is referred to as "tail-head linkage" (Longacre 1968(Longacre : 8-9, 1985Thompson & Longacre 1985: 209-210;de Vries 2005) or as a "bridging device" (Dixon 2009: 8).…”
Section: Sentences and Their Sequencingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This type of event report is in fact very common in the Papuan languages of Melanesia, as well as some Austronesian ones that perhaps adopted this strategy through language contact (cf. van Staden & Senft, 2001), and is possibly related to the general avoidance of having more than one full noun phrase or more than two overt (pronominal) arguments per predicate-argument structure so that all ditransitive actions and all actions involving manipulation of multiple objects are distributed over more than one predicate (de Vries, 2003;Du Bois, 1985, 1987Heeschen, 1998). In this interpretation of granularity, languages can be shown to be different in where they place the boundaries for event report.…”
Section: Partitioning Events In Languagementioning
confidence: 99%