Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
1947
DOI: 10.1080/00223980.1947.9917318
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Toward a Cultural Theory of Consonance

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
0
3

Year Published

1978
1978
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
0
22
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…It has also seemed plausible that consonance might not be rooted in acoustics at all, and is instead the arbitrary product of enculturation [23] -listeners might simply learn to like specific chords that are prevalent in the music of their culture. This notion is fueled in part by the use of the equal-tempered scale in modern music, in which consonant intervals only approximate integer ratios (Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has also seemed plausible that consonance might not be rooted in acoustics at all, and is instead the arbitrary product of enculturation [23] -listeners might simply learn to like specific chords that are prevalent in the music of their culture. This notion is fueled in part by the use of the equal-tempered scale in modern music, in which consonant intervals only approximate integer ratios (Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As Lundin (1947) In Experiment 2, each triad was allocated a different position in the short sequence IV-V-I in both major and minor keys (major and minor triads appeared only in sequences in major or minor keys respectively). The appearance of an augmented or diminished chord is expected in certain progressions, since both chords have certain "expected" harmonic functions.…”
Section: Present Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is thought that the perception of C/D depends not just on the sensory features of chords, but also on the musical context in which the interval or chord is presented. If this is the case, then factors such as: the principles of tonality; the musical context in which the chord appears; the listener's musical schema; the listener's familiarity with the style of music; and the music's acoustic properties may all play a part in shaping C/D perception (Cazden, 1980;Gardner & Pickford, 1943;Krumhansl, 1990;Lundin, 1947;Terhardt, 1984). Gardner and Pickford (1943,1944) conducted a study to demonstrate the influence of musical context, in which they employed various chords (details not given) in different contexts and keys as musical stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, cultural (Cazden, 1980;Fritz et al, 2009;Lundin, 1947;Vassilakis, 2005) and music-theoretical (Krumhansl, 1990) norms restrict the prevalence of inharmonic intervals within most tonal music repertoires (Huron, 1994), making dissonant moments unexpected and bewildering relative to their consonant counterparts (Costa, Bitti, & Bonfiglioli, 2000). This bewildering quality of dissonant music might further contribute to the information complexity of dissonant intervals, producing cognitive demands on the listener that consonant, harmonic music does not.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%