1991
DOI: 10.3758/bf03212222
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Tonal strength and melody recognition after long and short delays

Abstract: In a continuous-ronning-memory task, subjects heard novel seven-note melodies that were tested after delays of 11 sec (empty) or 39 sec (filled). Test items were transposed to new pitch levels (to moderately distant keys in the musical sense) and included exact transpositions (targets), samecontour lures with altered pitch intervals, and new-contour lures. Melodies differed in tonal strength (degree of conformity to a musical key) and were tonally strong, tonally weak, or atonal. False alarms to same-contour l… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
60
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 85 publications
(68 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
8
60
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Although we have derived tonal magnitude as a power transformation of the standardized key profile, on reflection it is clear that tonal magnitude is in many ways comparable to the more familiar concept of tonal strength. Research on tonal strength has occupied a central place in work on musical cognition, with such investigations extensively studying, for example, the consequences of varying tonal strength on the processing of and memory for musical passages (e.g., Croonen, 1994Croonen, , 1995Cuddy, Cohen, & Mewhort, 1981;Cuddy, Cohen, & Miller, 1979;Cuddy & Lyons, 1981;Dowling, 1978Dowling, , 1991. In addition, this work has identified characteristics that make a passage tonally strong, with music heard as tonally strong if it (a) is diatonic, in that it is composed primarily of pitches of the diatonic set; (b) begins and ends on the tonic; and (c) exhibits cadential structure (e.g., contains a sequence of chords built on Pitch Class 0, followed by Pitch Class 7, and ending on Pitch Class 0).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although we have derived tonal magnitude as a power transformation of the standardized key profile, on reflection it is clear that tonal magnitude is in many ways comparable to the more familiar concept of tonal strength. Research on tonal strength has occupied a central place in work on musical cognition, with such investigations extensively studying, for example, the consequences of varying tonal strength on the processing of and memory for musical passages (e.g., Croonen, 1994Croonen, , 1995Cuddy, Cohen, & Mewhort, 1981;Cuddy, Cohen, & Miller, 1979;Cuddy & Lyons, 1981;Dowling, 1978Dowling, , 1991. In addition, this work has identified characteristics that make a passage tonally strong, with music heard as tonally strong if it (a) is diatonic, in that it is composed primarily of pitches of the diatonic set; (b) begins and ends on the tonic; and (c) exhibits cadential structure (e.g., contains a sequence of chords built on Pitch Class 0, followed by Pitch Class 7, and ending on Pitch Class 0).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The tone set used for the auditory stimuli was slightly different from before, consisting of 220 Hz, 246.9 Hz, 261.6 Hz, 277.2 Hz, and 329.6 Hz (i.e., the musical notes A, B, C, C#, and E, respectively). As with the previous tone set, we used these tones because they avoid familiar melodies (Dowling, 1991). Additionally, this new tone set spans a smaller frequency range (220 Hz to 329.6 Hz, as opposed to 261.6 Hz to 493.8 Hz).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other researchers also proposed that specific intervals function as additional perceptual cues (e.g., Huovinen, 2002;Vos, 1999). Moreover, characteristics other than specific intervals have been indicated as additional cues for key perception, for example: two and three tone transitions (Krumhansl, 1979(Krumhansl, , 1990(Krumhansl, , 2000; a grouping of consecutive pitches on the basis of pitch proximity (Deutsch, 1984); a combination of specific intervals (e.g., Vos, 2000); the last tone of a sequence (Lamont, 1998;Parncutt & Bregman, 2000); both the pitches of the opening tone and the last tone of a sequence (Cuddy, Cohen, & Mewhort, 1981;Dowling, 1991); a combination of specific pitches (e.g., Bharucha, 1984;Cuddy, et al, 1981;Povel & Jansen, 2001, 2002; and pitch salience and sensory memory decay (Huron & Parncutt, 1993).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%