2012
DOI: 10.1093/police/pas027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

To Challenge, or not to Challenge? Best Practice when Interviewing Vulnerable Suspects

Abstract: This article examines to what extent police investigators can reliably question a vulnerable suspect's account when the evidence base for appropriate questioning styles for this particular vulnerable group is limited. We examine a simulated interview to demonstrate how difficult it is to challenge discrepancies in a vulnerable suspect's account. It is argued from both linguistic and psychological perspectives that certain question formats may lead to acquiescence, cognitive overload, and confusion for the susp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
30
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
30
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Intellectually disabled suspects are more vulnerable to give in to leading questions ("interrogative suggestibility"), react more compliant to authority figures ("compliance"), and produce more memories that are imagined or distorted ("confabulation"; Gudjonsson & Joyce, 2011;Scheyett et al, 2009). Hence, police officers must take precautions when interrogating suspects with intellectual disability (Gudjonsson, 2003;Kassin et al, 2010;O'Mahony, Milne, & Grant, 2012) by, for example, asking questions in a simple and clear way, using short sentences and speaking more slowly, and avoiding leading questions and interruptions (Gudjonsson & Joyce, 2011;Herrington & Roberts, 2012). Police officers, however, are not particularly apt at detecting individuals with intellectual disabilities (Gudjonsson, 2003;Parton, Day, & White, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Intellectually disabled suspects are more vulnerable to give in to leading questions ("interrogative suggestibility"), react more compliant to authority figures ("compliance"), and produce more memories that are imagined or distorted ("confabulation"; Gudjonsson & Joyce, 2011;Scheyett et al, 2009). Hence, police officers must take precautions when interrogating suspects with intellectual disability (Gudjonsson, 2003;Kassin et al, 2010;O'Mahony, Milne, & Grant, 2012) by, for example, asking questions in a simple and clear way, using short sentences and speaking more slowly, and avoiding leading questions and interruptions (Gudjonsson & Joyce, 2011;Herrington & Roberts, 2012). Police officers, however, are not particularly apt at detecting individuals with intellectual disabilities (Gudjonsson, 2003;Parton, Day, & White, 2004).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is no universal definition of what constitutes a vulnerable witness (Bull, ). However, those who might be considered vulnerable include child witnesses (Bull, ), individuals with mental illnesses and personality disorders (O'Mahony, Milne, & Grant, ), and individuals with intellectual disabilities (G. Gudjonsson & Joyce, ). For example, individuals with an intellectual disability may be more susceptible to leading questions (Bowles & Sharman, ) and misleading questions (Henry & Gudjonsson, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Without support they may not provide an account that is as complete, coherent and accurate as is possible and this may lead to unreliable police interviews (Gudjonsson, 2010). There is little, if any, guidance available to police officers on how to effectively challenge discrepancies in an account provided by a vulnerable suspect (O'Mahony et al, 2012). Similar difficulties may be found in the courtroom when persons with vulnerabilities provide oral testimony.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%