2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0996-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Titchener’s ⊥ in context 1—delimited, discrete monomotif patterns, line arrangements, and branching patterns

Abstract: Three experiments tested the effects of the presence of nontarget ⊥s on Titchener's (1901) ⊥-illusion. Experiment 1 used patterns of four separate ⊥s, Experiment 2 used branching patterns in which four ⊥s were stuck together, and Experiment 3 used patterns of four triangles or four beehive forms for which the ⊥ could be seen as a skeleton. Three independent samples of 12 observers each had to haptically indicate the lengths of target lines and verbally judge the relative lengths of the two lines of target ⊥s. … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, the net percent amount of the illusion was 2.41 %, which is quite in the range of what had been seen with individual, nondissected ⊥s (Landwehr, 2009(Landwehr, , 2014, as well as with ⊥s arranged in discrete, cyclically symmetric 4-⊥ patterns (Landwehr, 2015a). When computed separately for the three gap sizes, the illusion amounts were 3.71 %, 1.43 %, and 2.00 %, for 3, 10, and 16 cm, respectively.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 73%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Thus, the net percent amount of the illusion was 2.41 %, which is quite in the range of what had been seen with individual, nondissected ⊥s (Landwehr, 2009(Landwehr, , 2014, as well as with ⊥s arranged in discrete, cyclically symmetric 4-⊥ patterns (Landwehr, 2015a). When computed separately for the three gap sizes, the illusion amounts were 3.71 %, 1.43 %, and 2.00 %, for 3, 10, and 16 cm, respectively.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…3): Across gap conditions, it may have been easier to judge the length of the ⊥'s upstroke than that of the ⊥'s cross-stroke. This idea can be related to the broader study of the effects of different contexts (Armstrong & Marks, 1997;Chapanis & Mankin, 1967;Landwehr, 2015aLandwehr, , 2015b or, more specifically, of "framing" (Houck, Mefferd, & Greenstein, 1972;Prinzmetal & Gettleman, 1993). The idea is particularly intriguing for the haptic data, since indications for the upstroke hardly differed at all across gap sizes, whereas those for the cross-stroke were critically affected (Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations