2006
DOI: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.318
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Three-month stability of delay and probability discounting measures.

Abstract: Psychopharmacologists are interested in delay and probability discounting because the tendency to discount the value of future and uncertain rewards has been linked with drug dependency. However, relatively little is known about the long-term stability of discounting measures typically studied in clinical psychopharmacology. To evaluate the stability of discounting over a 3-month period, the authors compared points of subjective equality (indifference points) with those collected from the same subjects 3 month… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

21
182
1
3

Year Published

2008
2008
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 231 publications
(207 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
21
182
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…However, the MID task shows a fairly high test-retest reliability of relevant brain areas during reward processing (Fliessbach et al, 2010), specifically in the right striatum in response to alcohol cues in alcohol-dependent patients (Schacht et al, 2011). In addition, behavioral variables such as risk taking (White et al, 2008) and delay discounting (Ohmura et al, 2006) were also shown to be relatively stable. Although none of these studies investigated the same tasks used in the present study, they report adequate test-retest reliabilities of risk taking and reward processing and thus indicate that Determinants of early alcohol use in adolescents F Nees et al our present results might not be dependent on differences in reliability between measures.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the MID task shows a fairly high test-retest reliability of relevant brain areas during reward processing (Fliessbach et al, 2010), specifically in the right striatum in response to alcohol cues in alcohol-dependent patients (Schacht et al, 2011). In addition, behavioral variables such as risk taking (White et al, 2008) and delay discounting (Ohmura et al, 2006) were also shown to be relatively stable. Although none of these studies investigated the same tasks used in the present study, they report adequate test-retest reliabilities of risk taking and reward processing and thus indicate that Determinants of early alcohol use in adolescents F Nees et al our present results might not be dependent on differences in reliability between measures.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, researchers have commonly identified those data sets with an R 2 less than some threshold (e.g., R 2 < 0, .3, .4, or .5) and eliminated these data sets from comparative analyses (e.g., Acheson, Richards, de Wit, 2007;Epstein et al, 2003;Green & Myerson, 1995;Hamidovic, Kang, & de Wit, 2008;Myerson & Green, 1995;Ohmura et al, 2006;McDonald, Schleifer, Richards, & de Wit, 2003;Reynolds, Richards, & de Wit;Richards, Zhang, Mitchell, & de Wit, 1999;Reynolds & Schiffbauer, 2004;Simpson & Vuchinich, 2000). An R 2 value is obtained by using nonlinear regression to fit the data to a mathematical discounting model.…”
Section: Use Of R 2 For Testing Model Fit and Data Eliminationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Again, the discounting data were analyzed using a hyperbolic function and the reported correlations ranged from .71 to .90. Ohmura, Takahashi, Kitamura, and Wehr (2006) conducted a similar procedure but increased the delay between the two completions of the discounting task to 3 months and also analyzed their data with both a hyperbolic function and with area under the discounting curve (Myerson, Green, & Warusawitharana, 2001). For the hyperbolic function, the correlation between tests dropped to .60.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%