2017
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/wernx
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Thoughts of death disrupt foresight: the 'ostrich bias'

Abstract: Humans have a predilection for optimistic personal scenarios when thinking of their future. They tend not to project stressful episodes into the future and are inclined to repress the idea of their vulnerability, to an extent that, when explicitly asked to think about their death, they use various cognitive strategies to deny it. In this study, we investigated the specific coping persons can use when required to construct personal future scenarios after imagining their own death. Our participants were asked to… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3
1

Relationship

0
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is because imagining an emotional future event can trigger emotions in the here-and-now, "as if" the event were really occurring (Damasio, 2009;Gilbert & Wilson, 2007) -allowing people to anticipate the delayed value of their current patience. Indeed, there are a number of reasons to believe that the relationship between episodic foresight and intertemporal decision-making might ultimately depend on the emotional valence of the thoughts, including the catalogue of differences between the processes and consequences of positively and negatively valenced foresight (see Barsics, Van der Linden, & D'Argembeau, 2015;de Vito, Neroni, Gamboz, Della Sala, & Brandimonte, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is because imagining an emotional future event can trigger emotions in the here-and-now, "as if" the event were really occurring (Damasio, 2009;Gilbert & Wilson, 2007) -allowing people to anticipate the delayed value of their current patience. Indeed, there are a number of reasons to believe that the relationship between episodic foresight and intertemporal decision-making might ultimately depend on the emotional valence of the thoughts, including the catalogue of differences between the processes and consequences of positively and negatively valenced foresight (see Barsics, Van der Linden, & D'Argembeau, 2015;de Vito, Neroni, Gamboz, Della Sala, & Brandimonte, 2014).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One of the earliest findings on prospection was the identification of a valence effect on the characteristics of future-oriented thoughts. Compared with representations of negative future events, positive future events are more frequent and imagined faster (MacLeod & Byrne, 1996;Newby-Clark & Ross, 2003), are associated with more sensorial details, clearer representations of contextual information, greater feelings of pre-experiencing, and include more social contents (D'Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004;de Vito, Neroni, Gamboz, Della Sala, & Brandimonte, 2014;Painter & Kring, in press;Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Compared with representations of negative future events, positive future events are more frequent and imagined faster (MacLeod & Byrne, 1996;Newby-Clark & Ross, 2003), are associated with more sensorial details, clearer representations of contextual information, greater feelings of pre-experiencing, and include more social contents (D'Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2004;de Vito, Neroni, Gamboz, Della Sala, & Brandimonte, 2014;Painter & Kring, in press;Rasmussen & Berntsen, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%