2013
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00836
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The venetian-blind effect: a preference for zero disparity or zero slant?

Abstract: When periodic stimuli such as vertical sinewave gratings are presented to the two eyes, the initial stage of disparity estimation yields multiple solutions at multiple depths. The solutions are all frontoparallel when the sinewaves have the same spatial frequency; they are all slanted when the sinewaves have quite different frequencies. Despite multiple solutions, humans perceive only one depth in each visual direction: a single frontoparallel plane when the frequencies are the same and a series of small slant… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4

Relationship

1
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 4 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Cross-correlation models exhibit performance consistent with minimal relative disparity matching (Goutcher and Hibbard, 2010; Vlaskamp et al, 2013), disparity gradient limits (Filippini and Banks, 2009) and coarse-scale luminance and contrast similarity matching (Anderson and Nakayama, 1994; Goutcher and Hibbard, 2010), even though such models do not explicitly apply these rules for matching. Our results show, however, that the similarity-based matching emerging from the cross-correlation model differs markedly from results for human observers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Cross-correlation models exhibit performance consistent with minimal relative disparity matching (Goutcher and Hibbard, 2010; Vlaskamp et al, 2013), disparity gradient limits (Filippini and Banks, 2009) and coarse-scale luminance and contrast similarity matching (Anderson and Nakayama, 1994; Goutcher and Hibbard, 2010), even though such models do not explicitly apply these rules for matching. Our results show, however, that the similarity-based matching emerging from the cross-correlation model differs markedly from results for human observers.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The upper left panel of Fig. 7B shows that local cross-correlation of the images yields equally high-correlation peaks for numerous horizontal disparities ( 39 ). The same behavior is observed in disparity-selective cortical neurons: When presented with periodic stimuli, V1 neurons exhibit multiple response peaks ( 54 ).…”
Section: Natural Disparity Statistics and Perceived Depth From Disparmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The nonuniform allocation is manifest by pairs of retinal points with special status. These corresponding-point pairs are special because (i) binocular matching solutions between the two eyes’ images are biased toward them ( 37 39 ); (ii) the region of single, fused binocular vision straddles them ( 40 , 41 ); and (iii) the precision of stereopsis is greatest for locations in space that project to those points ( 42 46 ). The horopter is the set of locations in 3D space that project onto corresponding retinal points and therefore comprise the locations of finest stereopsis ( 47 , 48 ).…”
Section: Corresponding Points and The Horoptermentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Good matching features are found equally on both sides of the edge, which makes the percept ambiguous. The repeating pattern created by interlacing produces an image that is similar to the wallpaper illusion (Brewster, 1844;Vlaskamp et al, 2013), in which a repeating pattern creates a perception of depth. The nearest neighbor rule (Arditi et al, 1981) states that the visual system fuses the closest matching features to minimize the absolute disparity; however, this rule applies only in the absence of other images.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%