2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The value of variety and scarcity across development

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
40
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
5
40
2
Order By: Relevance
“…In sharp contrast to children's relative indifference to scarcity per se, we have found evidence for a powerful variety preference in young children (Echelbarger & Gelman, ). By variety, we mean selecting differences among items within sets.…”
Section: Implications For Object Valuecontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…In sharp contrast to children's relative indifference to scarcity per se, we have found evidence for a powerful variety preference in young children (Echelbarger & Gelman, ). By variety, we mean selecting differences among items within sets.…”
Section: Implications For Object Valuecontrasting
confidence: 80%
“…Second, it concurred with previous studies’ analytic strategy, thus again, allowing a more straightforward comparison to their findings. Third, following Echelbarger and Gelman’s () argument, setting chance at 50% provided a more conservative estimate for the presence of a scarcity bias, reasoning that children’s decision about which sticker to choose was clearly between two types of stickers, rather than among 12 individual stickers.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This change evidently required further modifications in the method, so as to make the task amenable to infants. For instance, rather than asking participants for their preferences for pictured objects (as done by Echelbarger & Gelman, ), we gave them an actual choice between objects they would get to keep (similar to Diesendruck et al, ; John et al, , in this respect). A second methodological change is that instead of presenting novel (as in Echelbarger & Gelman) or unknown (as in John et al) objects for children to choose from, we presented familiar objects that varied on a particular characteristic (i.e., balls that varied in color, or stickers of different types of balls).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are at least three reasons to think that children's choices, like adults', might be influenced by the groupings of options when children are tested with age-appropriate versions of paradigms that do elicit partition dependence reliably in adults. First, there is evidence that children diversify across options like adults (e.g., Read & Loewenstein, 1995), so at least one strategy leading to partition dependence-diversification bias-is present in childhood (see also Echelbarger & Gelman, 2017). Second, children as young as four flexibly reason about entities belonging to multiple categories (see Nguyen & Girgis, 2014, for a review), so children might draw on category groupings rather than concrete choice options when employing a diversification strategy.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%