2013
DOI: 10.1080/00222895.2012.747483
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Utility of Vision During Action: Multiple Visuomotor Processes?

Abstract: Recently, D. Elliott et al. (2010) asserted that the current control phase of a movement could be segregated in multiple processes, including impulse and limb-target regulation processes. The authors aimed to provide further empirical evidence and determine some of the constraints that govern these visuomotor processes. In 2 experiments, vision was presented or withdrawn when limb velocity was above or below selected velocity criteria. The authors observed that vision provided between 0.8 and 0.9 m/s significa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These main effects were in line with previous findings differentiating FV from NV conditions (e.g., Heath, 2005;Khan, Lawrence, Franks, & Buckolz, 2004;Tremblay, Hansen, Kennedy, & Cheng, 2013;cf. Bernier et al, 2006).…”
Section: Conventional Measures Of Online Feedback Utilizationsupporting
confidence: 81%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These main effects were in line with previous findings differentiating FV from NV conditions (e.g., Heath, 2005;Khan, Lawrence, Franks, & Buckolz, 2004;Tremblay, Hansen, Kennedy, & Cheng, 2013;cf. Bernier et al, 2006).…”
Section: Conventional Measures Of Online Feedback Utilizationsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Additionally, Elliott and Hansen computed their R 2 -type analyses from fewer trials per condition (i.e., 20) and through correlations at the occurrences of kinematic markers in the primary movement axis (i.e., peak acceleration, peak velocity, and peak deceleration; see also Heath, 2005). In contrast, in the present study we utilized a greater number of trials (i.e., 30) and normalized MT proportions (i.e., 25 % MT , 50 % MT , and 75 % MT ; see also Tremblay et al, 2013). Thus, it may be that such analyses are more sensitive regarding assessments of the percentage of MT (cf.…”
Section: A Comparison Of Methods: Effect Sizesmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…When vision is occluded, individuals display larger maximum grip apertures (Jackson, Jackson & Rosicky, 1995;Jakobson & Goodale, 1991;Rand, Lemay, Squire, Shimansky & Stelmach, 2007), slower movement times (Schettino, Adamovich & Poizner, 2003;Winges, Weber & Santello, 2003), and a decrease in task accuracy, to the degree that the hand often collides with the target object (Wing, Turton & Fraser, 1986) or misses the target completely (Babinsky, Braddick & Atkinson, 2012). In contrast, in the presence of vision, individuals show improved endpoint accuracy (Westwood, Heath & Roy, 2003), correct object size scaling (Keefe & Watt, 2009), and enhanced movement regulation (Saunders & Knill, 2003;Tremblay, Hansen, Kennedy & Cheng, 2013). Not surprisingly, vision also plays a critical role in hand preference for grasping.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Instructions to be accurate facilitated visual processing but 47 inhibited non-visual processing associated with reaches. 48 control as a multi-faceted construct (e.g., Tremblay, Hansen, Kennedy, & Cheng, 2013). 69…”
Section: Public Significance Statement: 44mentioning
confidence: 99%