2013
DOI: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
61
0
5

Year Published

2014
2014
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
1
61
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…This suggests that the developmental factors that result in a greater proportion of constraint-seeking questions across age groups may not be the same as those that we successfully manipulated within age groups. In particular, the shift in constraint-seeking questions across age groups could reflect more general developmental changes, such as those in the ability to organize, cluster, and represent information at different hierarchical levels (Hollister Sandberg, Huttenlocher, & Newcombe, 1996), or in cognitive flexibility (Anderson, 2002;Legare et al, 2013). It could also relate to more general developmental changes in language and working memory.…”
Section: Relationships To Prior Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This suggests that the developmental factors that result in a greater proportion of constraint-seeking questions across age groups may not be the same as those that we successfully manipulated within age groups. In particular, the shift in constraint-seeking questions across age groups could reflect more general developmental changes, such as those in the ability to organize, cluster, and represent information at different hierarchical levels (Hollister Sandberg, Huttenlocher, & Newcombe, 1996), or in cognitive flexibility (Anderson, 2002;Legare et al, 2013). It could also relate to more general developmental changes in language and working memory.…”
Section: Relationships To Prior Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For children, who are often surrounded by more knowledgeable peers and adults, asking questions is especially important for testing and extending their developing understanding of the world (Gopnik & Meltzoff, 1997;Gopnik, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 1999;Gopnik & Wellman, 1994;Harris, 2012;Piaget, 1954; see also Graesser & McMahen, 1993;Graesser & Olde, 2003). We know that young children ask domain-appropriate questions (Callanan & Oakes, 1992;Greif, Kemler Nelson, Keil, & Guiterrez, 2006;Hickling & Wellman, 2001), have reasonable expectations about which responses count as answers to their questions (Frazier, Gelman, & Wellman, 2009), and can use the answers they receive to solve problems (Chouinard, 2007;Legare, Mills, Souza, Plummer, & Yasskin, 2013). We also know that children's questions are responsive to the statistics of their environment in that they preferentially question reliable informants (Mills, Legare, Bills, & Mejias, 2010;Mills, Legare, Grant, & Landrum, 2011) and target informative cues (see Nelson, Divjak, Gudmundsdottir, Martignon, & Meder, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These researchers have found that when children encounter novel objects and phenomena in playful situations, they ask questions (e.g., Legare, Mills, Souza, Plummer, & Yasskin, 2013) and entertain various hypotheses about their use (e.g., Legare 2012Legare , 2014, formulate "experiments" and play more to disambiguate possible causes or effects (e.g., Cook, Goodman, & Schulz, 2011;Schulz & Bonawitz, 2007), and learn about the causal structure of objects through their own interventions (e.g., Sobel & Sommerville, 2010). For instance, in an experimental study, Legare (2012) found that when preschoolers encountered outcomes inconsistent with their prior experiences of whether or not particular blocks made a pair of boxes light up (in the classic "blicket detector" paradigm), their exploratory behaviors were related to the causal explanations they provided.…”
Section: Play As a Context For Scientific Thinkingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The tendency to seek and generate explanations is so pervasive and compelling that some psychologists have posited a ‘drive to explain’ . At very early ages, children generate appropriate domain‐specific explanations and use questions effectively to elicit explanations from others as a means of acquiring new knowledge . Given that young children generate and seek out explanations, how might explanations benefit causal learning?…”
Section: Explanation and Exploration In Children's Causal Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%