1979
DOI: 10.1016/0023-9690(79)90058-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The time course of learned helplessness, inactivity, and nociceptive deficits in rats

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
31
0
1

Year Published

1981
1981
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
2
31
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Because many of the behavioral and physiological effects of inescapable shock dissipate within this time interval (Maier et al, 1980;Seligman et al, 1975), forgetting cannot be solely responsible for time-dependent variations in helplessness. Thus, even though the presence of the pretreatment odor in the test apparatus is a necessary condition for several measures of helplessness to be observed 24 h after inescapable shock (Coen, 1985;Minor & LoLordo, 1984;Williams, 1987), some other factor must contribute to the production of and recovery from helplessness.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Because many of the behavioral and physiological effects of inescapable shock dissipate within this time interval (Maier et al, 1980;Seligman et al, 1975), forgetting cannot be solely responsible for time-dependent variations in helplessness. Thus, even though the presence of the pretreatment odor in the test apparatus is a necessary condition for several measures of helplessness to be observed 24 h after inescapable shock (Coen, 1985;Minor & LoLordo, 1984;Williams, 1987), some other factor must contribute to the production of and recovery from helplessness.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most importantly, behavioral impairment usually recovers within 72 h after exposure to inescapable shock (Maier, Coon, McDaniel, Jackson, & Grau, 1980;Seligman, Rosellini, & Kozak, 1975). This timedependent variation suggests either that helplessness effects are partially mediated by a nonassociative process, or that the odor-shock association established during pretreatment is readily forgotten (see Minor & LoLordo, 1984, for a thorough discussion).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The presentation of inescapable shock has been shown to interfere with the ability of a variety of speciesto later learn a novel response to escape shock (e.g., Anisman, Suissa, & Sklar, 1980;Maier, Albin, & Testa, 1973;Overmier& Seligman, 1967) or to' learn an appetitive operant task (Rosselini, 1978). Transituational deficits resulting from inescapable shock have also been found in tests of nonassociative or unlearned behaviors, such as nonreinforced shuttlebox running (e.g., Maier, Coon, McDaniel, Jackson, & Grau, 1979) and tail flicks to nociceptive stimuli (e.g., .…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The presentation of inescapable shock has been shown to interfere with the ability of a variety of speciesto later learn a novel response to escape shock (e.g., Anisman, Suissa, & Sklar, 1980;Maier, Albin, & Testa, 1973;Overmier& Seligman, 1967) or to' learn an appetitive operant task (Rosselini, 1978). Transituational deficits resulting from inescapable shock have also been found in tests of nonassociative or unlearned behaviors, such as nonreinforced shuttlebox running (e.g., Maier, Coon, McDaniel, Jackson, & Grau, 1979) and tail flicks to nociceptive stimuli (e.g., that shock-elicited aggression reflects attack or aggressive tendencies has been vehemently challenged on a number of grounds by Blanchard and his col- . First, the specific behaviors typically seen during shock-elictied aggression are very similar to those shown by colony intruder rats under attack and do not resemble the fighting pattern of a dominant male resident in an established colony.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In mice and rats, experiencing inescapable shock has been shown to interfere with the subsequent acquisition of escape responses (e.g., Anisman, Suissa, & Sklar, 1980), discrimination learning (Jackson, Alexander, & Maier, 1980), and appetitive operants (e.g., Rosellini, 1978;Rosellini, DeCola, & Shapiro, 1982). Transituational deficits produced by inescapable shock have also been found in tests of nonassociative or unlearned responses, such as nonreinforced shuttlebox running (Maier, Coon, McDaniel, Jackson, & Grau, 1979), shock-elicited fighting (Maier, Anderson, & Lieberman, 1972), general activity , and specific forms of exploratory behavior (e.g., Bruto & Anisman, 1983). In addition, inescapable shock has been found to result in opioid-and nonopioid-mediated analgesia to nociceptive stimuli (see Maier et al, 1983, for a review of this literature), and to induce increases in corticosteroids and acetylcholine and decreases in norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin (e.g., Anisman, This research was supported by USPHS Research Grant lROlNH4381S-{)1.…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%