2006
DOI: 10.1080/02796015.2006.12087976
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Taped-Problems Intervention: Increasing Multiplication Fact Fluency Using a Low-Tech, Classwide, Time-Delay Intervention

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
27
0
6

Year Published

2010
2010
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

4
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 82 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
2
27
0
6
Order By: Relevance
“…This is useful information for teachers because matching interventions to specific student deficits in a customized manner results in greater student growth during the intervention (Burns et al, 2015). Previous research shows that interventions such as cover–copy–compare (Codding, Chan-Iannetta, Palmer, & Lukito, 2009), incremental rehearsal (Burns, 2005), and taped problems (McCallum, Skinner, Turner, & Lee, 2006; Poncy, Skinner, & Jaspers, 2007) are generally effective in addressing computation deficits (Codding, Hilt-Panahon, Panahon, & Benson, 2009), as are interventions that incorporate the concrete–representational–abstract framework for teaching computation (Flores, Hinton, & Strozier, 2014; Mancl, Miller, & Kennedy, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is useful information for teachers because matching interventions to specific student deficits in a customized manner results in greater student growth during the intervention (Burns et al, 2015). Previous research shows that interventions such as cover–copy–compare (Codding, Chan-Iannetta, Palmer, & Lukito, 2009), incremental rehearsal (Burns, 2005), and taped problems (McCallum, Skinner, Turner, & Lee, 2006; Poncy, Skinner, & Jaspers, 2007) are generally effective in addressing computation deficits (Codding, Hilt-Panahon, Panahon, & Benson, 2009), as are interventions that incorporate the concrete–representational–abstract framework for teaching computation (Flores, Hinton, & Strozier, 2014; Mancl, Miller, & Kennedy, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In learning basic math skills, meeting the accuracy measure is as important as displaying it fluently (Cates and Ryhmer, 2003;McCallum et al, 2006;Shapiro, 2011). Fluency in basic math skills is answering a math fact automatically in verbal or written form quickly and accurately the moment it is read (Cressey and Ezbicki, 2008;Hinton et al, 2014;Johnson and Layng, 1996;Stocker and Kubina, 2017).…”
Section: Recommended Citationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…By reducing the demand on working memory, fluent recall of basic facts enables students to devote more attention to the overall purpose of a mathematics problem (Burns, Kanive, & DeGrande, 2012;Burns et al, 2016;Neill, 2008;Sweller, Ayres, & Kalyuga, 2011), which facilitates better understanding (Gross, Duhon, Shutte, & Rowland, 2016). Compared to their less fluent peers, students who are fluent with the recall of basic facts also enjoy more opportunities to respond to complex mathematics tasks (McCallum, Skinner, Turner, & Saecker, 2006), show an increased willingness to exert effort, and increased student motivation (Codding, Chanlannetta, Palmer, & Lukito, 2009). Despite the importance of fluent basic facts recall, many students find learning multiplication facts difficult (Steel & Funnell, 2001;Tait-McCutcheon, Drake, & Sherley, 2011), and as the mathematics curriculum increases in complexity, those less fluent are more likely to fall further behind their peers; a phenomenon commonly referred to as the Matthew effect (Merton, 1968).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%