2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-011-0090-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The striking similarities between standard, distractor-free, and target-free recognition

Abstract: It is often assumed that observers seek to maximize correct responding during recognition testing by actively adjusting a decision criterion. However, early research by Wallace (Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 4:441–452, 1978) suggested that recognition rates for studied items remained similar, regardless of whether or not the tests contained distractor items. We extended these findings across three experiments, addressing whether detection rates or observer confidence changed whe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
28
0
3

Year Published

2011
2011
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
(89 reference statements)
3
28
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Participants' resistance to within-list criterion shifting might be a partial result of inherent bias tendencies that anchor shifting behavior. Similarly, the remarkable unwillingness of participants to exercise appropriate levels of bias even when they are aware that test lists are composed only of targets or lures (J. C. Cox & Dobbins, 2011) might be explained in part by such an anchoring effect. Even when manipulations such as feedback, instructional motivation, and payoffs are successful in moving response bias, such shifts are usually suboptimal (e.g., Aminoff et al, 2012), another potential influence of trait bias.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Participants' resistance to within-list criterion shifting might be a partial result of inherent bias tendencies that anchor shifting behavior. Similarly, the remarkable unwillingness of participants to exercise appropriate levels of bias even when they are aware that test lists are composed only of targets or lures (J. C. Cox & Dobbins, 2011) might be explained in part by such an anchoring effect. Even when manipulations such as feedback, instructional motivation, and payoffs are successful in moving response bias, such shifts are usually suboptimal (e.g., Aminoff et al, 2012), another potential influence of trait bias.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The early amplitude 'unexpected recognition' effect could therefore reflect the recollection of contextual information, which engenders high certainty of prior encounter and thus is experienced as unexpected or surprising when one expects new materials. Since recollection is usually not triggered by new materials (Cox & Dobbins, 2011;Dobbins, 2014;Mickes, Hwe, Wais, & Wixted, 2011), this would explain why an analogous pattern doesn't arise when new items are unexpectedly encountered.…”
Section: Specificity Of the Unexpected Recognition Responsementioning
confidence: 99%
“…One recognition memory paradigm, explored most recently by Cox and Dobbins (2011), takes the relative frequencies of signal or noise stimulus presentations to an extreme. They asked subjects to rate how old probe words felt on a 6-point memory strength scale when test lists consisted of either all old words, all new words, or a standard 50 -50 combination of both.…”
Section: Recognition Memory: Other Issuesmentioning
confidence: 99%