1994
DOI: 10.1007/bf00233990
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics

Abstract: Earlier studies have suggested that the size of an object to be grasped influences the time taken to complete a prehensile movement. However, the use of cylindrical objects in those studies confounded the effects of object size-extent orthogonal to the reach axis-and object width-extent along the reach axis. In separating these effects, the present study demonstrates that movement time is not affected by manipulation of object size, as long as the latter does not approach the maximal object size that can be gr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
126
1
3

Year Published

1996
1996
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 198 publications
(145 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
15
126
1
3
Order By: Relevance
“…The work that is most similar to ours is that of Bootsma et al [2], who studied how prehensile movements are affected by movement amplitude, target size, and target width. The result of their work however, is not a model that describes the prehensile motion completely.…”
Section: Background and Motivationsupporting
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The work that is most similar to ours is that of Bootsma et al [2], who studied how prehensile movements are affected by movement amplitude, target size, and target width. The result of their work however, is not a model that describes the prehensile motion completely.…”
Section: Background and Motivationsupporting
confidence: 56%
“…There are several other properties that may impact the model, such as the width [2] and the weight of the artifact, whether it is a container that contains a liquid that may be spilled, the target's fragility, and others. While we believe that these factors may play a role in the model's behavior, we also believe that most of the artifacts that are presented in TUIs are well represented by our choice of the tested artifact.…”
Section: Limitations Of the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has been the focus of more than 200 published studies (nearly 150 of which have been published in the last 10 years). The equation has been shown to hold with prehension movements (e.g., Bootsma, Marteniuk, MacKenzie, & Zaal, 1994), mouse cursor movements (e.g., Lin, Radwin, & Vanderheiden, 1992), rotational movements (e.g., Abrams, Meyer, & Kornblum, 1990), head movements (e.g., Jagacinksi & Monk, 1985), and foot movements (e.g., Hoffmann, 1991). The equation holds underwater (Kerr, 1973), in near zero-gravity environments (Jungling, Bock, & Girgenrath, 2002), with microscopic targets (Langolf, Chaffin, & Foulke, 1976), with imaginary movements (Sirigu et al, 1996), and with targets that have illusory IDs (e.g., Fischer, 2001).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The weight of the object is normally estimated based on available visual information It means that the object can be properly "nestled" in the "containing capacity" of the hand. The prehensile adaptability of the hand [11,12] corresponds with the unlimited variability in size and shape of objects and instruments. It has been shown that increasing object width lowers the spatial accuracy demands on an object, permitting a faster movement to emerge.…”
Section: Anatomy Of Prehensilitymentioning
confidence: 99%