2012
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046619
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Role of the Caudal Superior Parietal Lobule in Updating Hand Location in Peripheral Vision: Further Evidence from Optic Ataxia

Abstract: Patients with optic ataxia (OA), who are missing the caudal portion of their superior parietal lobule (SPL), have difficulty performing visually-guided reaches towards extra-foveal targets. Such gaze and hand decoupling also occurs in commonly performed non-standard visuomotor transformations such as the use of a computer mouse. In this study, we test two unilateral OA patients in conditions of 1) a change in the physical location of the visual stimulus relative to the plane of the limb movement, 2) a cue that… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

5
38
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 84 publications
5
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Furthermore, unlike prism adaptation studies, the current study used a tool-use setting where visual targets and visual feedback were displayed on the vertical plane and hand movements were made on the horizontal plane, thereby requiring an additional coordinate transformation to link the hand and visual information [39]. Different visuomotor processes underlying reaching between natural and tool-use settings were revealed recently [49,62,63]. Moreover, the current task required controls of both the hand’s initial trajectory direction and stopping at the target, whereas prism adaptations often require mainly the control of the hand’s initial trajectory direction, such as throwing a ball [59] or rapid pointing being mechanically stopped as the finger lands on the target [60].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, unlike prism adaptation studies, the current study used a tool-use setting where visual targets and visual feedback were displayed on the vertical plane and hand movements were made on the horizontal plane, thereby requiring an additional coordinate transformation to link the hand and visual information [39]. Different visuomotor processes underlying reaching between natural and tool-use settings were revealed recently [49,62,63]. Moreover, the current task required controls of both the hand’s initial trajectory direction and stopping at the target, whereas prism adaptations often require mainly the control of the hand’s initial trajectory direction, such as throwing a ball [59] or rapid pointing being mechanically stopped as the finger lands on the target [60].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such egocentric guidance of conflicting visual and proprioceptive information in peripheral space [11,33] and online updating required following target displacement [25-27,29,30,60] are primarily affected from dorsal stream damage. This suggests that the OA deficit includes an impaired integration of conscious awareness of eye-centered metrics with transient online representations of limb-centered metrics.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The rationale to define a hand trajectory as corrective or as erroneous was as follows (Fig. 2): (1) we first obtained the distribution of all the finger endpoints for the undisplaced trials by calculating the ellipsoid 95% confidence interval (CI) (Granek et al, 2012;Messier and Kalaska, 1999). Trajectories of displaced trials in which the indexfinger endpoint position was within this CI were considered as erroneous trajectories (Pisella et al, 2000).…”
Section: Behavioral Analysismentioning
confidence: 99%