1992
DOI: 10.3758/bf03199659
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of syllabic and orthographic properties of letter cues in solving word fragments

Abstract: The present research examined the role of phonological and orthographic properties of cues in mediating the retrieval of words from the mental lexicon. The task required subjects to resolve fragmented words when provided with semantically related cues (e.g., spiteful: DIC ). Phonological properties of the letter cues were manipulated such that the letters either corresponded to the syllables (e.g., DIC in vindictive) or nonsyllables (NDI) in the word. Orthographic properties of the letter cues were manipulated… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This is relevant to the finding in the visual word recognition literature that “ TL ” words (words that can produce another word by transposing adjacent internal letters, e.g., SLAT / SALT ) take longer to be recognized than matched control words, suggesting that the presentation of SLAT also activates SALT (Andrews, 1996; Chambers, 1979). Thus, the memory block paradigm (and, more broadly, the fragment completion task) may offer visual word recognition researchers novel ways to study the links between words and their components (for an example of such a study, see Srinivas, Roediger, & Rajaram, 1992).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is relevant to the finding in the visual word recognition literature that “ TL ” words (words that can produce another word by transposing adjacent internal letters, e.g., SLAT / SALT ) take longer to be recognized than matched control words, suggesting that the presentation of SLAT also activates SALT (Andrews, 1996; Chambers, 1979). Thus, the memory block paradigm (and, more broadly, the fragment completion task) may offer visual word recognition researchers novel ways to study the links between words and their components (for an example of such a study, see Srinivas, Roediger, & Rajaram, 1992).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to the authors, readers store rules of spelling (identity and position of legal consonant and vocalic clusters, see also Gibson, 1965Gibson, , 1971) and this orthographic knowledge on letter co-occurrences enables readers to extract perceptual units without relying on phonological information. Similarly, Seidenberg (1987) proposed that letter cluster frequencies are prominent cues for word parsing into perceptual units (see also Adams, 1981;Srinivas, Roediger, & Rajaram, 1992). In particular, Seidenberg (1987) suggested that illusory conjunction effects were determined by the relative frequencies of adjacent bigrams, and more specifically by a bigram trough pattern, that is, a low frequency bigram surrounded by higher frequency ones on both sides.…”
Section: Polysyllabic Word Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a recent experiment, Spataro, Mulligan, Longobardi, and Rossi-Arnaud (2012) showed a similar interaction in the implicit task of word-fragment completion, which is primarily based on the retrieval of the orthographic properties of the studied words (Srinivas, Roediger, & Rajaram, 1992). They replicated the finding that the amount of repetition priming (i.e., higher completion rates for fragments corresponding to studied than to unstudied items) was greater for LA than for EA words.…”
mentioning
confidence: 85%