1977
DOI: 10.3758/bf03206076
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The role of structural redundancy in the perception of visual targets

Abstract: Most visual images contain "gross shapes" which are highly correlated with less perceptable "details," e.g., the gross outline of a head and details such as eyes, ears, etc. The role of such structural redundancy in perception is considered both from a "syntactical" and "spatialfrequency" view of structure. An experiment is reported and evaluated in terms of a simple mathematical model which allows one to isolate the influence of several factors in the perceptual process, particularly the observer's actual sen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

1
64
0

Year Published

1980
1980
1993
1993

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
(32 reference statements)
1
64
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The suggestion that the observer assigns weights to display locations according to their probabilistic priorities is consistent with a class of models that can be referred to as "weighted integration" (Kinchla, 1977) or "firstorder integration" (M. L. Shaw, 1982) models. The "order 1 code" X for a given location is a "sensory" representation of the stimulus that is influenced by the type of stimulus presented (target or nontarget), its energy, and so forth, and, possibly, by the amount of attention allocated to this source.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…The suggestion that the observer assigns weights to display locations according to their probabilistic priorities is consistent with a class of models that can be referred to as "weighted integration" (Kinchla, 1977) or "firstorder integration" (M. L. Shaw, 1982) models. The "order 1 code" X for a given location is a "sensory" representation of the stimulus that is influenced by the type of stimulus presented (target or nontarget), its energy, and so forth, and, possibly, by the amount of attention allocated to this source.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…First, the segmentation of an object into its component parts is often far more complicated than the segmentation of a word into its constituent letters (see Pomerantz, 1981). Second, a somewhat related problem is that a method for quantifying the level of contextual constraint (i.e., structural redundancy) for a given component part of a familiar object is not readily available (see Kinchla, 1977). Thus, it seems that a successful evaluation of the relationship between visual-context effects and redundancy in object perception requires the development of new experimental paradigms.…”
Section: Redundancy and Context Effects In Object Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, when studying visual-context effects with familiar everyday objects, an additional source for subjects' knowledge of structural redundancies, namely preexperimental knowledge, needs to be considered. As pointed out by Kinchla (1977), subjects may employ their knowledge ofa correlation between familiar visual images and their parts (i.e., structural redundancy) to infer, rather than actually "see," those parts when the objects are presented for a very brief duration. In fact, familiar everyday objects are highly redundant.…”
Section: Redundancy and Context Effects In Object Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As mentioned above, there is a high degree of redundancy among the structural levels of a scene (Kinchla, 1977;Navon, 1981;Palmer, 1975). Because of this high correlation, or nonorthogonality, the information acquired from one level can potentially be used to infer or predict information at another level.…”
Section: The Present Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, recognizing a stove allows one to predict a kitchen scene is present, and vice versa. (It was for this reason that Kinchla, 1977, developed orthogonal bilevel stimuli to investigate processing of stimulus structure.) To manipulate attention to structural levels effectively, it was necessary to remove or minimize the effects of the correlation between structurallevels at the critical parts of the scene (i.e., the location of the cued object, or the entire scene itself).…”
Section: The Present Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%