1997
DOI: 10.1080/016909697386871
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Role of Lexical Heads in Parsing: Evidence from German

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
70
1
1

Year Published

1998
1998
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 97 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
70
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…If the effect of left±right contingencies can be substantiated in further research, this may have important bearings on the hotly debated issue of head-driven parsing (for a recent overview of the literature, with novel experimental data, see Kamide & Mitchell, 1999;Konieczny, Hemforth, Scheepers & Strube, 1997). The U-Space model exempli®es strictly head-driven parsing: phrasal nodes (S, NP, PP, etc.)…”
Section: Discussion: Evaluating the Uni®cation Spacementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…If the effect of left±right contingencies can be substantiated in further research, this may have important bearings on the hotly debated issue of head-driven parsing (for a recent overview of the literature, with novel experimental data, see Kamide & Mitchell, 1999;Konieczny, Hemforth, Scheepers & Strube, 1997). The U-Space model exempli®es strictly head-driven parsing: phrasal nodes (S, NP, PP, etc.)…”
Section: Discussion: Evaluating the Uni®cation Spacementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subject) by the ®nite verb. Konieczny et al (1997), however, argue that this view of parsing is incorrect. German-language data obtained in their laboratory (see Hemforth, Konieczny & Strube, 1993) indicate that the oft-reported preference to interpret clause-initial NPs as the subject is measurable before the verb has been encountered: if clause-initial NPs are explicitly marked as non-nominative they consume slightly more processing time than NPs that are unambiguously nominative or have ambiguous case marking.…”
Section: Discussion: Evaluating the Uni®cation Spacementioning
confidence: 99%
“…For each region, we report first-pass reading times (summed duration of all fixations from first fixating within a region before a saccade from it -referred to as gaze duration for regions containing only one word, such as the postremnant region in the present experiment), total reading times (summed duration of all fixations in a region), and regression-path reading times (summed duration of all fixations from the time a region is first entered from the left until it is first exited to the right). Regression-path reading time (also called go-past reading times; Konieczny, Hemforth, Scheepers, & Strube, 1997;Liversedge, Paterson, & Pickering, 1998;Rayner & Duffy, 1986) includes fixations made to re-inspect earlier portions of text and provides an indication of early processing difficulty along with time spent re-inspecting text to recover from such difficulty (e.g., Liversedge et al, 1998;Rayner & Duffy, 1986). Analyses of reading times for each region excluded trials in which no fixations were made within that region and where reading times were above 5000 ms.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some researchers simply record first-pass reading time and then examine the pattern of regressions out of the target region. Others have argued for regression-path durations analyses (Konieczny, Hemforth, Scheepers, & Strube, 1997) or cumulative region reading time analyses (Brysbaert & Mitchell, 1996). With these analyses, reading time represents the sum of all fixations starting with the first fixation in a region and ending with the first forward saccade past the region under consideration 3 (see Liversedge, Paterson, & Pickering, 1998, for further discussion of these issues).…”
Section: What Is the Best Measure Of Processing Time?mentioning
confidence: 99%