2017
DOI: 10.1038/s41562-017-0219-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The risk elicitation puzzle

Abstract: Social and cultural historians have long used legal records to shed light on those otherwise lost to the historical record: the poor, the disenfranchised, youths, and women. This special issue seeks to interrogate what analytical value an explicit engagement with the emerging field of the "History of Emotions" can bring to explorations of law and emotions. In this Introduction, I suggest that working with a more methodologically reflexive understanding of emotions, and how they can be analyzed in concrete hist… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
178
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 206 publications
(221 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
15
178
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Adjusted Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) score as a function of the size at which the balloon exploded in Trial 1 (i.e., the first experienced explosion point; data from the Basel‐Berlin Risk Study; Frey et al, ; Pedroni et al, ). This analysis only includes participants who actually experienced an explosion in the first trial (i.e., 765 of all 1,507 participants).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Adjusted Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) score as a function of the size at which the balloon exploded in Trial 1 (i.e., the first experienced explosion point; data from the Basel‐Berlin Risk Study; Frey et al, ; Pedroni et al, ). This analysis only includes participants who actually experienced an explosion in the first trial (i.e., 765 of all 1,507 participants).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One interesting avenue toward a toolbox and taxonomy of risk preference measures is theory-driven task construction and decomposition using computational or neural methods that can disentangle risk preference from cognitive demands or other individual characteristics (Wallsten, Pleskac, and Lejuez 2005;Helfinstein et al 2014). However, we suspect that computational and neural methods offer no panacea for the lack of temporal stability and convergent validity of the currently available behavioral measures Pedroni et al 2017).…”
Section: A Look Aheadmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, the correlations among the nine different behavioral measures were substantially weaker (r = 0.08) than those among the 29 self-report measures (r = 0.20), even though the latter intentionally capture risk preference in diverse domains, such as financial, health, recreational, and social. The correlations between behavioral measures were not increased when parameters from specific decision models, such as expected utility theory or cumulative prospect theory, were used to describe individual's choices (Pedroni, Frey, Bruhin, Dutilh, Hertwig, and Rieskamp 2017). We also conducted a psychometric analysis using a bifactor model that directly accounts for shared variance across all measures with a single factor, leaving any residual variance to be captured by yet other specific, orthogonal factors.…”
Section: Convergent Validitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This might have limited our ability to replicate our previous result on the elevation of probability weighting (Ligneul et al, 2013) and more generally our ability to uncover true differences between groups or drug conditions. Furthermore, individual risk preferences have been shown to vary substantially across tasks, a phenomenon known as the “risk elicitation puzzle,” partly attributable to inconsistent decision strategies across tasks (Pedroni et al, 2017). This observation warrants some caution regarding the generalizability of the present findings, which could in part be driven by the specific demands of the task that we used.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%