2014
DOI: 10.1007/s11192-014-1354-z
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The reviewer in the mirror: examining gendered and ethnicized notions of reciprocity in peer review

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
20
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
20
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A few studies have suggested that female reviewers are more likely to recommend rejection (Borsuk et al 2009;Wing et al 2010) or that female editors are more likely to reject papers (Gilbert et al 1994), and at least one study has found an interaction between reviewer and author gender, with female reviewers reviewing female-authored papers more favourably (Lloyd 1990). However, this interaction was not observed in other studies (Petty, Fleming & Fabrigar 1999;Walker et al 2015) and most analyses of journal peer review data find no effect of reviewer or editor gender on review scores or manuscript decisions (Kliewer et al 2005;Bornmann & Daniel 2007;Grod et al 2008;Isenberg, Sanchez & Zafran 2009;Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto 2014;Walker et al 2015; note: Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto 2014 conclude that 'female reviewers gave lower reviews than male reviewers' in their study but clearly state in their Results that this result is not statistically significant). Notably, the studies that report gender bias in journal peer review data sets are generally older than the studies reporting no bias, possibly reflecting changing social attitudes towards women in science (Walker et al 2015).…”
Section: R E V I E W E R G E N D E R a F F E C T S A S P E C T S O F mentioning
confidence: 59%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…A few studies have suggested that female reviewers are more likely to recommend rejection (Borsuk et al 2009;Wing et al 2010) or that female editors are more likely to reject papers (Gilbert et al 1994), and at least one study has found an interaction between reviewer and author gender, with female reviewers reviewing female-authored papers more favourably (Lloyd 1990). However, this interaction was not observed in other studies (Petty, Fleming & Fabrigar 1999;Walker et al 2015) and most analyses of journal peer review data find no effect of reviewer or editor gender on review scores or manuscript decisions (Kliewer et al 2005;Bornmann & Daniel 2007;Grod et al 2008;Isenberg, Sanchez & Zafran 2009;Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto 2014;Walker et al 2015; note: Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto 2014 conclude that 'female reviewers gave lower reviews than male reviewers' in their study but clearly state in their Results that this result is not statistically significant). Notably, the studies that report gender bias in journal peer review data sets are generally older than the studies reporting no bias, possibly reflecting changing social attitudes towards women in science (Walker et al 2015).…”
Section: R E V I E W E R G E N D E R a F F E C T S A S P E C T S O F mentioning
confidence: 59%
“…; Bornmann & Daniel ; Grod et al . ; Isenberg, Sanchez & Zafran ; Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto ; Walker et al . ; note: Demarest, Freeman & Sugimoto conclude that ‘female reviewers gave lower reviews than male reviewers’ in their study but clearly state in their Results that this result is not statistically significant).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…what is dubbed cognitive particularism [40], whereby scholars harbor preferences for work and 48 ideas similar to their own [41]. Evidence of this process has been reported in peer review in the 49 reciprocity (i.e., correspondences between patterns of recommendations received by authors and 50 patterns of recommendations given by reviewers in the same social group) between authors and 51 reviewers of the same race and gender [42] (see also [43,44]). Reciprocity can exacerbate or Starting from the left, an initial submission is first given an initial decision of encourage or reject, and if encouraged, continues through the first full review and subsequent rounds of revision.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This means that looking at referee behavior could potentially help to reveal scientist misbehavior or situations where referees could benefit from their gatekeeping role at the expense of editors and/or authors (e.g., Bornmann, Weymuth, & Daniel, ; Lamont, ; García, Rodriguez‐Sánchez & Fdez‐Valdivia, ); it could also inform us about the nature of the social norms of reviewing and so help to counterbalance possible bias (e.g., Bornmann & Daniel, ; Demarest, Zhang, & Sugimoto, ; Lee, Sugimoto, Zhang, & Cronin, ; Mahoney, ; Sugimoto & Cronin, ), especially in multidisciplinary journals where heterogeneous norms coexist and could conflict (e.g., Huutoniemi, Thompson Klein, Bruun, & Hukkinen, ). Observing the behavior of referees is even more important for multidisciplinary journals, in which problems of incompatible standards of judgment can arise.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%