2008
DOI: 10.1080/15250000701795572
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Representation of Geometric Cues in Infancy

Abstract: There is evidence that, from an early age, humans are sensitive to spatial information such as simple landmarks and the size of objects. This study concerns the ability to represent a particular kind of spatial information, namely, the geometry of an enclosed layout-an ability present in older children, adults, and nonhuman animals (e.g., Cheng, 1986;Hermer & Spelke, 1996). Using a looking-time procedure, 4.5-to 6.5-montholds were tested on whether they could distinguish among the corners of an isosceles trian… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
39
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
2

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 83 publications
1
39
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Tested on the same tasks, young US children showed some abstract intuitions of Euclidean geometry, particularly when reasoning about the properties of straight lines on a plane; however, they were less able to adapt their responses to different surfaces, and they failed to produce angles that respected the constancy of the sum of three angles in a triangle. This last finding echoes recent reports of failure to use angles in toddlers and young preschool children (29,30), which came as a surprise given the success of children in navigational triangle completion tasks (15) and their ability to process angles in 2D small-scale figures from infancy (31)(32)(33). Although more research is needed to explore why the young children sometimes fail with angles, one possibility is that the adult concept of abstract angle, applying equally well to planar figures and 3D shapes and to small-and large-scale figures, is a mental construct arising during childhood (34).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…Tested on the same tasks, young US children showed some abstract intuitions of Euclidean geometry, particularly when reasoning about the properties of straight lines on a plane; however, they were less able to adapt their responses to different surfaces, and they failed to produce angles that respected the constancy of the sum of three angles in a triangle. This last finding echoes recent reports of failure to use angles in toddlers and young preschool children (29,30), which came as a surprise given the success of children in navigational triangle completion tasks (15) and their ability to process angles in 2D small-scale figures from infancy (31)(32)(33). Although more research is needed to explore why the young children sometimes fail with angles, one possibility is that the adult concept of abstract angle, applying equally well to planar figures and 3D shapes and to small-and large-scale figures, is a mental construct arising during childhood (34).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…Cross-cultural research with indigenous populations and studies using visual attention paradigms with infants suggest that they are also among the most psychologically intuitive. Knowledge of core geometric concepts (e.g., points and lines) and the ability to perform basic (nonsymbolic) arithmetic computations (e.g., addition and subtraction) have been documented in cultures without access to formal math instruction (2, 7) and even in preverbal infants (57,58). Such intuitions, it is believed, rest on perceptual properties of the visual system and universal spatial experiences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's sensitivity to distance and directional relations in a navigation task predicted their use of a map to find targets in a 3D array with surfaces at distinct distances; their sensitivity to properties of object shapes in a form analysis task predicted their use of the same map to find targets in a 3D array with corners of distinct angles. This pattern of findings provides evidence that in their untutored interpretations of symbolic maps, children flexibly recruit the core geometric representations that emerge in infancy (10,11,30,31), are shared by other animals (2,7,32,33), and are used by children and adults throughout their lives.…”
mentioning
confidence: 84%