2012
DOI: 10.1080/0013838x.2012.700587
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Relevance of Constructions for the Interpretation of Modal Meaning: The Case ofMust

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The proportion of epistemics is substantially lower than in the corpus studies on adult speech mentioned in the introduction (Ruppenhofer & Rehbein, 2012, Rubinstein et al, 2013, Marasović et al, 2016, see also Coates, 1983, Klages & Römer, 2002, Hacquard & Wellwood, 2012, Baker et al, 2012. For some of these studies, the difference might be due to the fact that they involve written language (Coates, 1983 andHaan, 2011 show that modal use differs substantially in written vs. spoken language). But even studies on spoken adult language show a higher proportion of epistemics: in the MASC corpus of American English, Marasović et al (2016) find 26% epistemics in almost 2,000 functional modals (n = 508, their Table 5, p.17).…”
Section: Interim Discussion On the Inputmentioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The proportion of epistemics is substantially lower than in the corpus studies on adult speech mentioned in the introduction (Ruppenhofer & Rehbein, 2012, Rubinstein et al, 2013, Marasović et al, 2016, see also Coates, 1983, Klages & Römer, 2002, Hacquard & Wellwood, 2012, Baker et al, 2012. For some of these studies, the difference might be due to the fact that they involve written language (Coates, 1983 andHaan, 2011 show that modal use differs substantially in written vs. spoken language). But even studies on spoken adult language show a higher proportion of epistemics: in the MASC corpus of American English, Marasović et al (2016) find 26% epistemics in almost 2,000 functional modals (n = 508, their Table 5, p.17).…”
Section: Interim Discussion On the Inputmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…In what follows we treat such uses as root (as opposed to epistemic). 18 The studies that come closest are DeHaan (2011), which looks at temporal and aspectual properties of must , and Marasovi ć et al (2016), which uses the modal lemma and temporal-aspectual properties in combination with other variables such as subject and negation to tag modal flavor automatically.Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jos/article/39/4/581/6676120 by guest on 31 March 2023…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To take must as an example, example (1) below shows must in its obligation meaning while ( 2) is an instance of the probability meaning. The underlined items in these two examples show co-textual features that tend to be associated with the respective meanings (Coates, 1983;Hunston, 2000;de Haan, 2012). In (1) an animate subject and an agentive verb are associated with the obligation meaning.…”
Section: Sampling -Checking Obligation Meaningmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although the scarce attention to pragmatics in constructionist accounts of modals (for exceptions, see e.g. Stefanowitsch 2003;Boogaart 2009;De Haan 2012;Boogaart & Fortuin 2016) might suggest that the integration of pragmatics in modal constructions is problematic, it is certainly possible to include pragmatic information in a modal construction's representation. After all, constructions can come with pragmatic specifications (for surveys, see Kay 2004;Nikiforidou 2009; the introduction in Bergs & Diewald 2009;Lee-Goldman 2011;Cappelle 2017; introduction to this issue).…”
Section: Introduction: Modal Constructions Lexically Restricted Satumentioning
confidence: 99%