1984
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1984.tb00798.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The relativity of ‘absolute’ judgements

Abstract: Suppose that, with rather few exceptions, the assignment of a number to a stimulus on one trial of a magnitude estimation or category judgement experiment serves as the point of reference for choosing an appropriate assignment on the next trial. This principle of relative judgement—relative to the immediate context—is developed to generate models for both magnitude estimation and category judgement experiments. It is applied, in particular, to the explanation of three, hitherto unrelated, phenomena: these are … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

6
123
1

Year Published

1996
1996
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 184 publications
(130 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
(27 reference statements)
6
123
1
Order By: Relevance
“…On the other hand, absolute accounts predict that data from experiments with unequally spaced stimuli should not be radically different from standard data. To illustrate the point, consider the relative judgment models of Laming (1984), Holland and Lockhead (1968), and Stewart et al (2005) and, for simplicity of the example, ignore sequential effects. These models depend critically on a single estimate for the difference between adjacent stimulus magnitudes.…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…On the other hand, absolute accounts predict that data from experiments with unequally spaced stimuli should not be radically different from standard data. To illustrate the point, consider the relative judgment models of Laming (1984), Holland and Lockhead (1968), and Stewart et al (2005) and, for simplicity of the example, ignore sequential effects. These models depend critically on a single estimate for the difference between adjacent stimulus magnitudes.…”
Section: Methods Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The theoretical debate has progressed mainly by pairwise comparison of particular absolute and relative models-for example, Marley and Cook (1984) versus Laming (1984); Petrov and Anderson (2005) versus Stewart et al (2005); and Stewart et al (2005) versus Brown et al (2008). There have been one or two attempts at a more general comparison, but these have proven less diagnostic than was hoped (see, e.g., Brown, Marley, & Lacouture, 2007;Stewart, 2007;Stewart et al, 2005, Experiment 2).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such an assumption is intuitive, but also appears in various forms across a range of psychological theories (e.g., Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992; see also relative judgment models of perceptual differences, Laming, 1984;Stewart, Brown & Chater, 2005).…”
Section: A Qp Theory Model For Constructive Measurementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One psychological assumption concerns the impact of introducing the second stimulus (i.e., a fixed rotation, relative to the initial state; Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992;Laming, 1984;Stewart et al, 2005).…”
Section: A Qp Theory Model For Constructive Measurementmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A model that uses only absolute processes is one where decisions are made about stimulus magnitudes on the basis of comparisons with some longer term referents (e.g., the context-coding component of Braida et al, 1984;Lacouture & Marley, 1995, 2004 or a longer-term frame of reference (e.g., Marley & Cook, 1984). On the other hand, a model that uses only relative processes (e.g., Laming, 1984;Lockhead, 2004;Stewart et al, 2005) posits that decisions are made using only comparisons with recent stimuli and responses. Range and set size effects have most often been attributed to absolute processes (e.g., Braida et al, 1984;Marley & Cook, 1984).…”
Section: Local Effects: Sequential Effect On Accuracy and Errorsmentioning
confidence: 99%