1933
DOI: 10.1080/00221309.1933.9920918
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Relative Susceptibility of Pleasant, Unpleasant, and Indifferent Material to Retroactive Inhibition

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

1935
1935
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Derry and Kuiper (1981) found that individuals are less likely to remember negative than positive descriptions of themselves. Positive words are also learned faster than negative words (Rychlak, 1966; Bunch and Wientge, 1933), and people judge positive phenomena more accurately than negative phenomena. Managers, for example, are much more accurate in rating subordinates’ competencies and proficiencies when they perform correctly than when they perform incorrectly (Gordon, 1970).…”
Section: Inherent Inclinations Toward the Positivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Derry and Kuiper (1981) found that individuals are less likely to remember negative than positive descriptions of themselves. Positive words are also learned faster than negative words (Rychlak, 1966; Bunch and Wientge, 1933), and people judge positive phenomena more accurately than negative phenomena. Managers, for example, are much more accurate in rating subordinates’ competencies and proficiencies when they perform correctly than when they perform incorrectly (Gordon, 1970).…”
Section: Inherent Inclinations Toward the Positivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Edwards (1969) found that individuals are less likely to remember negative than positive descriptions of themselves. Positive words are also learned faster than negative words (Bunch & Wientge, 1933;Rychlak, 1966), and people judge positive phenomena more accurately than negative phenomena. Managers, for example, are much more accurate in rating subordinates' competencies and proficiencies when they perform correctly than when they perform incorrectly (Gordon, 1970).…”
Section: Natural Tendencies Toward the Positivementioning
confidence: 99%
“…words into a single category of "emotional," companng the leaming rate on these words with the wcM-ds judged as being indifferent (Bunch & Wientge, 1933, Jwies, 1929, Lynch, 1932, Smith, 1921 There fcJlowed a host of studies dunng the 1930s which asked the subject to recall or othenvise leam a hst of words that had been prerated lô thers (l e , nomothetically) as to pleasantness (Carter, 1935, 1937, Carter bcjooes, 1937, White, 1936, White & Pbwell, 1936, White & Rathff, 1934, or that the subject had rated fw himself OT herself, (that is, ldiographically (Silyerman & Cason, 1934, Stagner, 1933, Thomson, 1930 The finding ofa leaming supenonty of jdeasant over un^deasant ami indifferent words continued to show through these collective results, particularly wlten the i»-ejudgments were handled ldiograplucally…”
Section: Emdencefrom Other Besearch Sourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A yiaMe thewy must draw empincal evidence frwn swirces that have not been generated by this specific outlook In the present section I will review yanous research findings that are readily subsumed by the tenets of LLT As noted aboye, findings on affection have been well documented, dating back at least to Tait (1913), who drew on Wundt's tl^ory of enwtions to demonstrate that pleasant words were easier to leam than unpleasant words Tolman (1917) was to find the same facilitaticm effect m a study of retroactive inhibition OccasMHially unjJeasant words were leamed more readily than wwds that were judged as "indifferent" by subjects This was unfortunate, because the practice then arose of himpmg jdeasant and unpleasant (liked vs dishked?) words into a single category of "emotional," companng the leaming rate on these words with the wcM-ds judged as being indifferent (Bunch & Wientge, 1933, Jwies, 1929, Lynch, 1932, Smith, 1921 There fcJlowed a host of studies dunng the 1930s which asked the subject to recall or othenvise leam a hst of words that had been prerated lô thers (l e , nomothetically) as to pleasantness (Carter, 1935, 1937, Carter bcjooes, 1937, White, 1936, White & Pbwell, 1936, White & Rathff, 1934, or that the subject had rated fw himself OT herself, (that is, ldiographically (Silyerman & Cason, 1934, Stagner, 1933, Thomson, 1930 The finding ofa leaming supenonty of jdeasant over un^deasant ami indifferent words continued to show through these collective results, particularly wlten the i»-ejudgments were handled ldiograplucally The g»ieral jwddem of affective mflueitte on leaming then fell from view for over a cfecade, until the "perceptual cfefense" studies refamfled an interest in this area W^k m the modem penod has focused jvimanly aa such frequency issues as I discussed m the p-evums section lypical problems addressed included whether the subject's affective assessments ({deasant-un^easant, good-bad, iilce-dislike) are truly in^pen-(fent of his or her extent oScoatact with a givoi term, saad the number of wwd-associates that a subject can prodxxce to an afifecbvely toned wwdstunulus (Cantcn;1935, CnHnwell, 1956ke, & Martin, 1961, Koen, 1962, Sarbin & Quenk, 1964, Staats, 1^9, Z^yctK, 1968 The dnft of these findmgs suggested that an aff<»^ve dimeasvm is at w(8-k m verbal meamng, o^ea asTelated with but still taiepeadesA from Cheese frequency measures of associatuH) value An especially int^esting study for present p...…”
Section: Emdencefrom Other Besearch Sourcesmentioning
confidence: 99%