2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.01.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The recognition without cued recall phenomenon: Support for a feature-matching theory over a partial recollection account

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

6
62
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(69 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
6
62
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Many researchers of recognition memory have long argued that recognition can occur solely on the basis of a general sense of familiarity (e.g., Diana, Reder, Arndt, & Park, 2006;Mandler, 1991Mandler, , 2008Ryals & Cleary, 2012;Yonelinas, 2002), and some have argued that familiarity detection itself may have an emotional component, possibly involving arousal (e.g., Goldinger & Hansen, 2005;Morris et al, 2008). Future research should examine whether the effects reported here involve an emotional component.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Many researchers of recognition memory have long argued that recognition can occur solely on the basis of a general sense of familiarity (e.g., Diana, Reder, Arndt, & Park, 2006;Mandler, 1991Mandler, , 2008Ryals & Cleary, 2012;Yonelinas, 2002), and some have argued that familiarity detection itself may have an emotional component, possibly involving arousal (e.g., Goldinger & Hansen, 2005;Morris et al, 2008). Future research should examine whether the effects reported here involve an emotional component.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Ryals and Cleary (2012) suggested that cue familiarity when target retrieval fails (e.g., Cleary, 2004;Cleary, Ryals, & Nomi, 2009) can be brought on by the type of feature-matching process described by global-matching models (e.g., Clark & Gronlund, 1996). Ryals and Cleary's cues were nonwords (e.g., foneheed) that potentially resembled studied words (e.g., forehead) graphemically.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In support of the feature-matching hypothesis, graphemic cues that resembled four studied words (but for which none of those studied words was recalled in response to the cue) received higher familiarity ratings than graphemic cues for which recall failed but that resembled only one studied word (which in turn received higher ratings than graphemic cues that did not resemble any studied words). In short, Ryals and Cleary (2012) found support for a featurematching account of RWCR when the cue resemblance to studied items is graphemic in nature.The present study is concerned with whether the featurematching approach can plausibly account for RWCR that is found when cue resemblance to studied items is semantic in nature. Cleary (2004) demonstrated that when participants studied words (e.g., cheetah) and were tested with semantically related cues (e.g., jaguar), participants gave higher ratings to those cues that semantically resembled studied words than to those that did not.…”
mentioning
confidence: 75%
“…Ryals and Cleary (2012) suggest that at least one basis for this experience may be a feature-matching process, whereby features (i.e., attributes) of the current situation are compared with those of representations stored in memory to produce a sense of familiarity that varies according to the degree of match. Ryals and Cleary (2012) used the example of recognizing the street sign for "Marston" as familiar because of its high degree of featurematch to a recently-seen street sign, "Morton." Despite failing to trigger recall of having recently passed a street sign for "Morton," the new sign "Marston" may still seem familiar because of its high degree of feature-overlap with the recently-encountered sign "Morton."…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation