2014
DOI: 10.1111/eth.12266
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Rate of Degradation of Chemical Cues Indicating Predation Risk: An Experiment and Review

Abstract: Many prey taxa use kairomones or alarm pheromones to assess the risk of predation in aquatic environments, and the rate at which these cues attenuate determines how precisely they indicate the local density of predators. We estimated the rate of degradation of chemical cues generated by Aeshna dragonfly larvae feeding on Rana temporaria tadpoles. The half-life of the cue was 35 h and was not influenced by whether it was aged in pond water or tap water or whether other tadpoles were present in the container in … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
41
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
2
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In a preliminary study, we confirmed that A. cyanea larvae do not defecate after being deprived of food for 3 days. It has been claimed that not only defecation may generate digestionreleased prey-borne cues (Brown et al 1995), but cues that label the predators as dangerous are known to degrade within 48 h or less (Peacor 2006;Ferrari et al 2008;Van Buskirk et al 2014).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In a preliminary study, we confirmed that A. cyanea larvae do not defecate after being deprived of food for 3 days. It has been claimed that not only defecation may generate digestionreleased prey-borne cues (Brown et al 1995), but cues that label the predators as dangerous are known to degrade within 48 h or less (Peacor 2006;Ferrari et al 2008;Van Buskirk et al 2014).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even if the duration of food restriction is known, it is not always clear that prey-borne cues are completely absent. The rate of degradation of pre-consumption prey-borne cues has been measured (Peacor 2006;Ferrari et al 2008;Van Buskirk et al 2014), but predators may defecate long after they consumed prey and digestion-released prey-borne cues may therefore persist. Observed prey responses may therefore not be attributed solely to continually released predator-borne cues (but see Petranka and Hayes 1998;Schoeppner and Relyea 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Olfactory signals differ from other forms of communication because they can continue to broadcast information for hours in the absence of the signaler, and can thus have a lasting impact on space use (Chivers et al 2013; Van Buskirk et al 2014; Campbell & Roberts 2015). Like other signal types, chemical cues attract (Durisko & Dukas 2013; Bett & Hinch 2015), repel (Goodale & Nieh 2012; Ibáñez et al 2012), and immobilize conspecifics (Brechbühl et al 2008; Fraker et al 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One example is habitat/host race formation, in which populations evolve ecological specialization in response to variation in habitat or host species (Kawecki 1997;Nosil et al 2005;Van Buskirk 2014). One example is habitat/host race formation, in which populations evolve ecological specialization in response to variation in habitat or host species (Kawecki 1997;Nosil et al 2005;Van Buskirk 2014).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the spatial structure of risk depends on details of prey risk assessment and predator behavior. Predator chemicals persist 8-40 h in lentic habitats (Van Buskirk et al 2014) and diffuse as much as 0.5-2 m (Turner and Montgomery 2003;Wisenden 2008;Takahara et al 2012). This is true when (1/n) > Vt2d, where n is the density of the predator, V is the velocity of the predator, t is the half-life of the signal indicating risk, and d is the distance over which the signal is detectable.…”
Section: For Plasticitymentioning
confidence: 99%