2017
DOI: 10.1007/s11166-017-9261-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The psychometric and empirical properties of measures of risk preferences

Abstract: We examine the psychometric and empirical properties of some commonly used survey-based measures of risk preferences in a population-based sample of 11,000 twins. Using a model that provides a general framework for making inferences about the component of measured risk attitudes that is not due to measurement error, we show that measurement-error adjustment leads to substantially larger estimates of the predictive power of risk attitudes, of the size of the gender gap, and of the magnitude of the sibling corre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
41
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 102 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
(96 reference statements)
2
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While this design removes the uncertainty associated with measuring time preferences, it potentially reduces the weight of each decision given that choices are hypothetical. 5 Finally, our Anchoring task is based on the Wheel-of-Fortune task introduced in Tversky and Kahneman (1974), where a randomly generated number was found to influence subjects' answers to an independent question. We created an abstract version of the task to allow for scalability in a repeated setting.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While this design removes the uncertainty associated with measuring time preferences, it potentially reduces the weight of each decision given that choices are hypothetical. 5 Finally, our Anchoring task is based on the Wheel-of-Fortune task introduced in Tversky and Kahneman (1974), where a randomly generated number was found to influence subjects' answers to an independent question. We created an abstract version of the task to allow for scalability in a repeated setting.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…von Gaudecker et al (2011), using both hypothetical and incentivized measures on a Dutch sample (using the CentER internet panel), find that women on average are less risktaking than men (1422 participants). 8 Beauchamp et al (2017) use a random sample (approximately 11,000 individuals) of the Swedish twin population (the sample of twins is similar to the general population on some selected characteristics). Also using the nonincentivized risk measure mentioned above, they find that male twins on average are more risktaking than female twins (only looking at same-sex twins).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on literature concerning general rather than health-specific risk attitude, individuals with a higher education are expected to be more risk seeking (Donkers et al, 2001;Schurer, 2015). Following literature in which health behaviour is associated with risk attitude (Anderson & Mellor, 2008;Beauchamp et al, 2017;Dieteren et al, 2020;Dohmen et al, 2011;Weber et al, 2002), it is expected that HRAS-13 scores are positively associated with unhealthy behaviours (i.e. smoking, poor nutrition, excessive drinking and limited physical exercise) of respondents.…”
Section: Validitymentioning
confidence: 99%