2021
DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051053
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Psychology of Reaching: Action Selection, Movement Implementation, and Sensorimotor Learning

Abstract: The study of motor planning and learning in humans has undergone a dramatic transformation in the 20 years since this journal's last review of this topic. The behavioral analysis of movement, the foundational approach for psychology, has been complemented by ideas from control theory, computer science, statistics, and, most notably, neuroscience. The result of this interdisciplinary approach has been a focus on the computational level of analysis, leading to the development of mechanistic models at the psychol… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
71
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 69 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 218 publications
0
71
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This invariance poses a challenge to the standard state-space model of sensorimotor adaptation where the rate and magnitude of learning are dependent on error size (Herzfeld, Vaswani, Marko, & Shadmehr, 2014; Marko, Haith, Harran, & Shadmehr, 2012; Shadmehr et al, 2010; Smith, Ghazizadeh, & Shadmehr, 2006). Thus, the current results add additional evidence pointing to the need for novel perspectives of adaptation, ones that do not assume adaptation to be sensitive to error size, but instead constrained by the limits of sensorimotor plasticity (Kim et al, 2018) or sensory biases (Heald, Lengyel, & Wolpert, 2020; Jonathan S. Tsay, Kim, et al, 2020).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 66%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This invariance poses a challenge to the standard state-space model of sensorimotor adaptation where the rate and magnitude of learning are dependent on error size (Herzfeld, Vaswani, Marko, & Shadmehr, 2014; Marko, Haith, Harran, & Shadmehr, 2012; Shadmehr et al, 2010; Smith, Ghazizadeh, & Shadmehr, 2006). Thus, the current results add additional evidence pointing to the need for novel perspectives of adaptation, ones that do not assume adaptation to be sensitive to error size, but instead constrained by the limits of sensorimotor plasticity (Kim et al, 2018) or sensory biases (Heald, Lengyel, & Wolpert, 2020; Jonathan S. Tsay, Kim, et al, 2020).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 66%
“…If the mismatch is small, this change will emerge in a gradual manner over trials and occurs outside the participant’s awareness, a phenomenon known as implicit sensorimotor adaptation. If the mismatch is large, this adaptive learning process may also be accompanied by more explicit adjustments in aiming (Kim, Avraham, & Ivry, 2020; McDougle, Ivry, & Taylor, 2016; Shadmehr, Smith, & Krakauer, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, the behavioral changes observed during sensorimotor learning do not arise solely from explicit strategy use. The behavioral change in such tasks is also driven by implicit adaptation, the adjustment in the sensorimotor map that occurs outside awareness and volitional control [13]. Indeed, in many contexts, especially those involving small perturbations, most of the learning is implicit [12,14,15].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…But in a more general sense, prediction errors do not necessarily require movement. For example, in classical conditioning, these errors are driven directly by unexpected sensory stimuli (Ohmae & Medina, 2015; Kim et al, 2020; Ito, 2007; Sears & Steinmetz, 1991; Rasmussen et al, 2008). Here, we wondered if passive movements could generate prediction errors in a similar manner, and if consistent exposure to these errors might increase sensitivity to error during active motor learning.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%