1980
DOI: 10.3758/bf03204304
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The primacyof visual information inthe analysis of letter strings

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

1981
1981
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Aside from Gibson's rule-governed conception, other studies based on a statistical approach showed that readers were sensitive to transitional probabilities between letters and that orthographic redundancy may facilitate letter and word perception (e.g., Anisfeld, 1964;Massaro, Taylor, Venezky, Jastrzembski, & Lucas, 1980;Morton, 1969;Singer, 1980). As underlined by Henderson (1982), most of those studies eschewed wholism and generally assumed no perceptual units larger than individual letters.…”
Section: Orthographic Redundancymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Aside from Gibson's rule-governed conception, other studies based on a statistical approach showed that readers were sensitive to transitional probabilities between letters and that orthographic redundancy may facilitate letter and word perception (e.g., Anisfeld, 1964;Massaro, Taylor, Venezky, Jastrzembski, & Lucas, 1980;Morton, 1969;Singer, 1980). As underlined by Henderson (1982), most of those studies eschewed wholism and generally assumed no perceptual units larger than individual letters.…”
Section: Orthographic Redundancymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Likewise, Pollatsek, Well, and Schindler (1975) found that latency to respond "different" to homophonic word pairs was no longer than latency to respond to nonhomophonic word pairs. Singer (1980), using artificial letters to represent orthographic and nonorthographic strings, imposed response deadlines and found that visual information preceded phonological information in the recognition of the pseudoword targets.…”
Section: Phonological and Orthographic Factorsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Script extensivity Many studies with artificial characters used restricted sets (e.g., 6-12 characters only; e.g., Bitan & Karni, 2004;Jeffrey & Samuels, 1967;Singer, 1980;Yoncheva et al, 2010). This could be sufficient for certain nonlinguistic studies, but it is definitely not valid when the aim is to closely reproduce situations of exposure to natural print.…”
Section: Why and How Devising A Set Of Artificial Characters?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The aim was no more to simulate reading acquisition per se (which is actually hardly possible, see Knafle & Legenza, 1978, for a discussion), but rather to examine the developmental course of letter string processing (e.g., acquisition of visual expertise in reading: ; development of high quality lexical representations: Hart & Perfetti, 2008; letter position coding: García-Orza, Perea, & Muñoz, 2010) or to finely investigate processes that occur during letter/word processing (e.g., effects of orthographic or graphotactic regularities: Samara & Caravolas, 2014;Singer, 1980;Mason & Katz, 1976;print-tosound consistency effects: Taylor, Plunkett, & Nation, 2011; influence of first language characteristics on the acquisition of a second language: Ehrich & Meuter, 2009;Meuter & Ehrich, 2012; influence of handwriting knowledge on letter recognition: Longcamp, Boucard, Gilhodes, & Velay, 2006). For such studies, the relevance of using unknown or artificial characters lies in the possibility of investigating issues in a Bpure^way, in the sense that the degree of familiarity with the script is fully controlled and that it is easier to avoid confounds that are inevitable with natural stimuli.…”
Section: Using Unknown and Artificial Characters: State Of The Artmentioning
confidence: 99%