2019
DOI: 10.18632/aging.102088
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The positive impacts of early-life education on cognition, leisure activity, and brain structure in healthy aging

Abstract: Education in people’s early lives are positively related to their cognitive function, but its modulating effects on detailed cognition domains, its interaction with leisure activities and the associated brain changes have yet to be investigated. This report used data from 659 cognitively normal community dwelling elderly who completed neuropsychological tests, leisure activities measurement, and 78 of them underwent structural and diffusion MRI scans. We found that: (i) the highly educated elderly had a better… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
26
0
3

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 56 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
26
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…This led to the conclusion that the changes of FA are more likely a result of the number of dives and are not due to the possibility that more experienced divers are too likely to be older. The absence of changes in the WM of controls is likely to be dependent on the relatively young age of the subjects (mean value 39.7 years ±7.22 SD), none of which reached 55 years (Chen et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This led to the conclusion that the changes of FA are more likely a result of the number of dives and are not due to the possibility that more experienced divers are too likely to be older. The absence of changes in the WM of controls is likely to be dependent on the relatively young age of the subjects (mean value 39.7 years ±7.22 SD), none of which reached 55 years (Chen et al, 2019).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) was used for baseline cognitive assessment. At 2-year and 4-year follow-up visits, standard clinical and neuropsychological testing batteries will be performed, [19][20][21][22][23][24] FOLLOW-UP ASSESSMENT Field personnel will be in frequent contact with the study population to determine whether they develop any cardiovascular/cerebrovascular events or cognitive decline. All participants will be followed up through face-to-face interviews by trained investigators at 2 years and 4 years.…”
Section: Cognitive Assessmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Prominent accounts of heterogeneity in neural and behavioral aging postulate that this is the case, arguing that education acts as a modifiable protective factor ( 1 ) or cognitive reserve ( 2 , 3 ) of human neurocognitive aging. However, findings from cross-sectional studies provide only inconclusive support for an association between education and neurocognitive level in aging ( 4 7 ), and the longitudinal support for an influence of education on age-related neurocognitive change is even more elusive. In fact, a comprehensive review recently concluded that level of education does not reliably influence the rate of cognitive decline in aging ( 8 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%