2017
DOI: 10.1086/692737
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Physiology of Framing Effects: Threat Sensitivity and the Persuasiveness of Political Arguments

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 13 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample size but our sample size is larger than those reported in previous publications. 1 , 7 , 26 28 , 33 Specific periods of data collection were: between October 27, 2014 and December 12, 2014 (protocol 1: N=105); June 24, 2015 and November 28, 2016 (protocol 2: N=106); and February 16, 2017 and January 30, 2018 (protocol 3: N=141). The full sample consists of students and non-student adults (N=352).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…No statistical methods were used to pre-determine sample size but our sample size is larger than those reported in previous publications. 1 , 7 , 26 28 , 33 Specific periods of data collection were: between October 27, 2014 and December 12, 2014 (protocol 1: N=105); June 24, 2015 and November 28, 2016 (protocol 2: N=106); and February 16, 2017 and January 30, 2018 (protocol 3: N=141). The full sample consists of students and non-student adults (N=352).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kevin Smith and John Hibbing — two of the lead authors of the Oxley et al paper — acknowledged the need for replication, as they wrote that the question whether their findings replicate cannot be known “until other laboratories have weighed in” 25 (p.3, emphasis added). The need for a direct replication is even more pressing since — to the best of our knowledge — the only published conceptual replications offer conflicting conclusions 26 28 — for details see Supplementary Results p.33.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another example is need for cognition (i.e., the extent to which people believe they enjoy thinking; Cacioppo & Petty, 1982), where advertising a product as relevant for those enjoy (vs. don't enjoy) intensive thinking enhances the appeal for those higher in this trait (Bakker, 1999; See et al, 2009). Other research has observed similar effects for dominance orientation (i.e., assertive messages for those high in dominance vs. diffident messages for those low; Moon, 2002), sensation seeking (i.e., unusual message structures for high sensation seekers vs. normal message structures for those low; Palmgreen et al, 2002; Self & Findley, 2010), future orientation (i.e., distant advantages/immediate disadvantages for those high in future orientation vs. immediate advantages/distant disadvantages for those low; Strathman et al, 1994; Tangari & Smith, 2012), and many other individual identities (e.g., Coe et al, 2017; Mannetti et al, 2010; Williams‐Piehota et al, 2004, for a review, see Dijkstra, 2008). Indeed, even self‐esteem has been effectively targeted: Advertising nonidealized (vs. idealized) content is more effective for consumers who view themselves unfavorably (Bian & Wang, 2015).…”
Section: Personalized Matchingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As discussed in Online Appendix Section 1, we identified seven existing studies of lay people that included measures of EDA responses to negative images and political ideology: Aarøe et al 2017, Coe et al (2017), Dodd et al (2012), Knoll et al (2015), Oxley et al (2008), Petersen et al (2015) and Smith et al (2012). Table 1 provides an overview of the studies, including their country location, sample size, types of images included (and those used to generate their measure of threat-sensitivity), the specific method for analyzing the physiological data, the included ideological measures, as well as our tests of the reliability and validity of the measures.…”
Section: Samplingmentioning
confidence: 99%