2008
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework

Abstract: After a long period of neglect, the phenomenology of action has recently regained its place in the agenda of philosophers and scientists alike. The recent explosion of interest in the topic highlights its complexity. The purpose of this paper is to propose a conceptual framework allowing for a more precise characterization of the many facets of the phenomenology of agency, of how they are related and of their possible sources. The key assumption guiding this attempt is that the processes through which the phen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
433
1
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 474 publications
(449 citation statements)
references
References 58 publications
(68 reference statements)
12
433
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Our observation that neonatal arm movements are a basic, primary form of intended action has important bearing on understanding the development of agency and intentionality. It stands in agreement with a growing philosophical psychological account that basic, embodied intentions are part‐and‐parcel of the core sense of self (Alcaro, Carta, & Panksepp, 2017; Delafield‐Butt & Gangopadhyay, 2013; Feinberg & Mallatt, 2016; Fuchs & Koch, 2014; Gallagher, 2000, 2005; Hohwy, 2007; Merker, 2007; Northoff & Panksepp, 2008; Pacherie, 2008; Stern, 2010; Trevarthen & Delafield‐Butt, 2017; Zahavi, 2005, 2006). This non‐verbal modality of expressive gesture made in communication with a caring and attentive other underpins preverbal, embodied intersubjective communication important for psychological development and health (Delafield‐Butt & Trevarthen, 2015; Di Paolo & De Jaegher, 2015; Rochat & Gallese, 2016; Trevarthen et al., 2015) and, in cases where the infant's mental health or development is threatened, may require professional assistance to support (Brazelton, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…Our observation that neonatal arm movements are a basic, primary form of intended action has important bearing on understanding the development of agency and intentionality. It stands in agreement with a growing philosophical psychological account that basic, embodied intentions are part‐and‐parcel of the core sense of self (Alcaro, Carta, & Panksepp, 2017; Delafield‐Butt & Gangopadhyay, 2013; Feinberg & Mallatt, 2016; Fuchs & Koch, 2014; Gallagher, 2000, 2005; Hohwy, 2007; Merker, 2007; Northoff & Panksepp, 2008; Pacherie, 2008; Stern, 2010; Trevarthen & Delafield‐Butt, 2017; Zahavi, 2005, 2006). This non‐verbal modality of expressive gesture made in communication with a caring and attentive other underpins preverbal, embodied intersubjective communication important for psychological development and health (Delafield‐Butt & Trevarthen, 2015; Di Paolo & De Jaegher, 2015; Rochat & Gallese, 2016; Trevarthen et al., 2015) and, in cases where the infant's mental health or development is threatened, may require professional assistance to support (Brazelton, 2006).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…That is, besides the sense of ownership, we also experience intentional movements as realisations of our agency (Bayne, 2008;Pacherie, 2008). It is through our body that we move and act, feed ourselves and satisfy our needs, and more abstractly, satisfy our drives and motivation.…”
Section: Limits Of the Modelmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(a) the goal state [or the end target position for the body also called a ''motor intention" or M-intention by Pacherie (2008).] This represents a target position of the body; (b) the motor commands, of which there are two copies, produced by the so called 'inverse model'.…”
Section: The Comparator Model Of the Sense Of Agency Over Bodily Actionsmentioning
confidence: 99%