2007
DOI: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.2.267
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The perversity of Inanimate Objects: Stimulus Control by Incidental Musical Notation

Abstract: Social cognition research suggests that incidental, environmental stimuli (e.g., business suits) can nonconsciously influence the degree to which behavioral dispositions (e.g., competitiveness) are expressed. Similarly, cognitive research suggests that incidental action-related objects (e.g., hammers) can prime action plans that then affect the speed with which a concurrent, intended action (e.g., power grip) is executed. However, whether incidental stimuli can instigate actions that run counter to one's curre… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
3
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
1
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Three of the scales were found to be highly correlated, creating a triadic relationship, with tempo as its pivotal quality. This is conformant with well-rehearsed evidence: early studies (Hevner, 1936 , 1937 , p. 625; Rigg, 1940 ) had observed that speed was a primary determinant of musical mood and listener response, and that variations in tempo invoked strong effects on affective responses. Budd ( 1985 ), similarly describes tempo as an “essential musical device for expression of emotion,” and a critical factor of musical interpretation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…Three of the scales were found to be highly correlated, creating a triadic relationship, with tempo as its pivotal quality. This is conformant with well-rehearsed evidence: early studies (Hevner, 1936 , 1937 , p. 625; Rigg, 1940 ) had observed that speed was a primary determinant of musical mood and listener response, and that variations in tempo invoked strong effects on affective responses. Budd ( 1985 ), similarly describes tempo as an “essential musical device for expression of emotion,” and a critical factor of musical interpretation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…For example, perceiving a cylinder unconsciously potentiates one's tendency to perform a power grip (see review in Ellis, 2009). In addition, it has been shown that in choice RT tasks, the mere presence of musical notation influences the responses of musicians but not of nonmusicians (Levine, Morsella, & Bargh, 2007;Stewart et al, 2003).…”
Section: Unconscious Activation Of Action Plans Through Supraliminal mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Rigg's experiments (1937, 1940a,b, cited in Rigg, 1964) might have only used five musical phrases, but a total of seven cues were manipulated in each of these examples ( tempo, mode, articulation, pitch level, loudness, rhythm patterns , and interval content ). He asked listeners to choose between happy and sad emotion categories for each excerpt, as well as further describe the excerpts using precise emotional expressions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%