2004
DOI: 10.1002/sce.20003
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The persistence of personal and social themes in context: Long‐ and short‐term studies of students' scientific ideas

Abstract: In this paper we will examine the persistence of "misconceptions." We used data from a longitudinal study of personal ideas in 24 students' thinking about ecological processes. The results show students often speaking about personal experiences dating from an early age, to which they had also referred in similar interviews conducted years before. These data are compared with results from a different study of middle school physics students' thinking about energy and steam engines. After the new learning had bee… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If students do not recognize and reject their own scientific misconceptions in favor of scientifically accurate explanations, they will simply accommodate new knowledge obtained in the classroom within their existing framework rather than correcting their misconceptions. Even if students recognize their misconceptions during the semester, if they are not given the opportunity and motivation to reject or modify them, the students will usually revert to their misconceptions some time after completion of the science course (Greene 1990;Wandersee et al 1989;Hellden and Solomon 2004;Mintzes et al 2000).…”
Section: The Role Of Misconceptions and The Anthropology Instructor'smentioning
confidence: 96%
“…If students do not recognize and reject their own scientific misconceptions in favor of scientifically accurate explanations, they will simply accommodate new knowledge obtained in the classroom within their existing framework rather than correcting their misconceptions. Even if students recognize their misconceptions during the semester, if they are not given the opportunity and motivation to reject or modify them, the students will usually revert to their misconceptions some time after completion of the science course (Greene 1990;Wandersee et al 1989;Hellden and Solomon 2004;Mintzes et al 2000).…”
Section: The Role Of Misconceptions and The Anthropology Instructor'smentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Such predispositions might exist because they are useful or appropriate in everyday life-that is, in contexts other than thinking about biological evolution-and children have picked them up there (cf. Helldén and Solomon's (2004) suggestion that students' ''life-world knowledge'', which is continuously reinforced in daily conversation, cannot be eradicated by science teaching), or they might simply be innate (as Gelman (2003), for example, suggested for essentialist thinking).…”
Section: Contextualizationmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…The first is that the misconceptions about evolution prevalent among scholars internationally at all educational levels, both before officially being taught the topic and often afterwards (for example, Bishop and Anderson 1990;Moore et al 2002) are also prevalent among South African pupils (Kagan and Sanders 2013;Lawrence 2015;Moore et al 2002;Mpeta 2013;Schroder 2012;Yalvac 2011). Having misconceptions is, in itself, problematic, but a more serious consequence is that erroneous ideas interfere with the success of further learning (Freyberg and Osborne 1985) and tend to be difficult to teach away (Helldén and Solomon 2004). The existence of misconceptions has serious consequences when evolution is meant to provide an underlying framework for explaining basic processes and phenomena in the biological sciences.…”
Section: Problems Motivating the Studymentioning
confidence: 99%