2008
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000908008866
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The perfective past tense in Greek child language

Abstract: This study examines the perfective past tense of Greek in an elicited production and an acceptability judgment task testing 35 adult native speakers and 154 children in six age groups (age range: 3 ; 5 to 8 ; 5) on both existing and novel verb stimuli. We found a striking contrast between sigmatic and non-sigmatic perfective past tense forms. Sigmatic forms (which have a segmentable perfective affix (-s-) in Greek) were widely generalized to different kinds of novel verbs in both children and adults and were o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

6
46
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
6
46
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus the performance of patient IS in Tsapkini et al 's (2002b) study indicates that sigmatic versus non-sigmatic forms may dissociate in the Greek perfective past-tense system. Contrasts between sigmatic and non-sigmatic past tense forms were also found in Varlokosta et al 's (2008) study of WS in Greek and in a recent acquisition study of the perfective past tense in Greek child language (Stavrakaki & Clahsen, 2009). As the performance of the participants with WS we examined for the present study will be compared to subgroups of typically-developing children from Stavrakaki & Clahsen (2009), we present a brief summary of this study.…”
Section: The Greek Past Tense: Acquisition and Processingsupporting
confidence: 54%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Thus the performance of patient IS in Tsapkini et al 's (2002b) study indicates that sigmatic versus non-sigmatic forms may dissociate in the Greek perfective past-tense system. Contrasts between sigmatic and non-sigmatic past tense forms were also found in Varlokosta et al 's (2008) study of WS in Greek and in a recent acquisition study of the perfective past tense in Greek child language (Stavrakaki & Clahsen, 2009). As the performance of the participants with WS we examined for the present study will be compared to subgroups of typically-developing children from Stavrakaki & Clahsen (2009), we present a brief summary of this study.…”
Section: The Greek Past Tense: Acquisition and Processingsupporting
confidence: 54%
“…Contrasts between sigmatic and non-sigmatic past tense forms were also found in Varlokosta et al 's (2008) study of WS in Greek and in a recent acquisition study of the perfective past tense in Greek child language (Stavrakaki & Clahsen, 2009). As the performance of the participants with WS we examined for the present study will be compared to subgroups of typically-developing children from Stavrakaki & Clahsen (2009), we present a brief summary of this study. Stavrakaki & Clahsen (2009) employed elicited production and acceptability judgment tasks to examine 35 adult native speakers of Greek and 154 typicallydeveloping Greek children in six age groups (age range: 3;5 to 8;5) on both existing and novel verb stimuli.…”
Section: The Greek Past Tense: Acquisition and Processingsupporting
confidence: 54%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Consider mixed errors in Spanish and Greek child language. Three-to-four-year old children have been reported to produce errors such as *pusí [I.put.PST] (correct: pus-e) in Spanish or *eplise instead of epline [washed.3SG] in Greek Stavrakaki & Clahsen 2009). In such errors, marked (irregular) stems of the corresponding verbs (e.g., pus-) are combined with regular inflectional suffixes (instead of irregular ones) indicating that children can manipulate stems and inflectional endings separately.…”
Section: Stems and Inflectional Classesmentioning
confidence: 99%