2000
DOI: 10.1093/jos/17.4.281
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Partitive Constraint in Optimality Theory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We presented (11) as our initial hypothesis because it is the grammatically strongest one, in the sense of making the grammar responsible for explaining the greatest range of data. It appears necessary for OT grammars to admit floating constraints in order to deal with multiple types of variation: synchronic adult variation in phonology (Nagy andReynolds 1997, Anttila 1998) and syntax/semantics (Anttila and Fong 2000), diachronic variation (Zubritskaya 1997, Anttila and Young-mee to appear), variation in child language (Legendre, Hagstrom, Todorova and Vainikka 2000) and (under our interpretation here) variation across lexical strata (Itô and Mester 1995b, etc.). With this mechanism already introduced within grammatical theory, the grammatically strongest hypothesis would appear to be that it accounts for any other type of variation.…”
Section: Final Grammar = Base Grammarmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We presented (11) as our initial hypothesis because it is the grammatically strongest one, in the sense of making the grammar responsible for explaining the greatest range of data. It appears necessary for OT grammars to admit floating constraints in order to deal with multiple types of variation: synchronic adult variation in phonology (Nagy andReynolds 1997, Anttila 1998) and syntax/semantics (Anttila and Fong 2000), diachronic variation (Zubritskaya 1997, Anttila and Young-mee to appear), variation in child language (Legendre, Hagstrom, Todorova and Vainikka 2000) and (under our interpretation here) variation across lexical strata (Itô and Mester 1995b, etc.). With this mechanism already introduced within grammatical theory, the grammatically strongest hypothesis would appear to be that it accounts for any other type of variation.…”
Section: Final Grammar = Base Grammarmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is therefore of some interest that the theory of variation explored here automatically provides a rationale for preferences in interpretation. This was first noted by Anttila and Fong (2000) who used this theory to derive preferences among the alternative readings of Finnish partitives.…”
Section: Deriving Patterns Of Ambiguitymentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Recall that the same ranking resulted in a categorical contrast in my cat/ Ã the cat of me. This reveals a fundamental property of quantitatively interpreted optimality-theoretic grammars: the same ranking may yield both categorical and quantitative effects, depending on the input (Anttila, 1997, in press;Anttila and Cho, 1998;Anttila and Fong, 2000; see Boersma and Hayes, 2001 for a somewhat different theory). The interpretation in (36) is an empirical hypothesis and as such possibly wrong.…”
Section: Deriving Patterns Of Variationmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Following ideas put forward by Antilla and colleagues (Antilla & Cho, 1998;Anttila & Fong, 2000) Bouma assumes underspecified, partial rankings that can be described by putting constraints in so-called strata. Using stratified grammars it is possible to achieve ambiguity in comprehension even if bidirectional optimization is taken into account.…”
Section: Grammarmentioning
confidence: 99%