1981
DOI: 10.2307/1129166
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Origins of Concept Formation: Object Sorting and Object Preference in Early Infancy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
26
0
1

Year Published

1987
1987
2010
2010

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 73 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
26
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…One object from each class was placed on the table, and the child was allowed to place the other six objects. As in comparable studies, all age groups engaged in some similarity-based grouping behavior (Nelson, 1973a;Ricciuti, 1965;Starkey, 1981), with younger infants producing simple one-class groupings (e.g., making a row of plates while ignoring the blocks) and older infants often producing two-class groupings (e.g., making a row of plates and a row of blocks), a process that requires comparing items to determine similarity and difference.…”
Section: Similarity In Perceptual Domainsmentioning
confidence: 54%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…One object from each class was placed on the table, and the child was allowed to place the other six objects. As in comparable studies, all age groups engaged in some similarity-based grouping behavior (Nelson, 1973a;Ricciuti, 1965;Starkey, 1981), with younger infants producing simple one-class groupings (e.g., making a row of plates while ignoring the blocks) and older infants often producing two-class groupings (e.g., making a row of plates and a row of blocks), a process that requires comparing items to determine similarity and difference.…”
Section: Similarity In Perceptual Domainsmentioning
confidence: 54%
“…Although this line of study properly belongs to the category of "parallel development" (since there is no telling which direction of influence applies) we have included it here because, like Premack's work, it bears on the relation between language about similarity and similarity processing. As discussed earlier, in the sequential touching task infants are given objects drawn from two identity classes, and their spontaneous touching and grouping patterns are observed (Nelson, 1973a;Ricciuti, 1965;Starkey, 1981). Sugarman (1982) found that 12-month-olds tended to group objects to form one identity group, while 24-to 36-month-olds tended to form two identity groups.…”
Section: Effects Of Language On the Relational Shiftmentioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For this reason, and to determine whether touching runs of three or four objects from the same category occurred by chance, a second measure of run length was carried out. In accordance with the analyses used in previous categorizing studies, infants who systematically touched objects from one category were termed single categorizers, and infants who systematically touched objects from both categories were termed dual categorizers.t The criterion for a categorizing run was the same as that used in previous research (e.g., Mandler et al, 1991 ;Starkey, 1981;Sugarman, 1983 ). A Monte Carlo program was used to determine the probability of occurrence of single and dual categorizing runs in 10,000 random draws.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Developmental studies suggest that there are important changes in children's tendency to sort objects into groups between one and two years of age (Ricciuti, 1965;Sugarman, 1983;Starkey, 1981). We hypothesized that there might be a relation between this cognitive change and the onset of the «naming explosion» -the first sharp increase in children's naming vocabulary that typically occurs at about 1.5 years of age.…”
Section: On Linking Infant Sensory-motor Development and Language Devmentioning
confidence: 97%